Skip to main content
Log in

Extracting Meaning from Sound: Nomic Mappings, Everyday Listening, and Perceiving Object Size from Frequency

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In developing a theoretical framework for the field of ecological acoustics, Gaver (1993b) distinguished between the experience of musical listening (perceiving sounds) and everyday listening (perceiving sources of sounds). Within the everyday listening experience, Gaver (1993a) proposed that the frequency of an object results from, and therefore specifies, the size of that object. The relation in which frequency and object size stand to one another is an example of a nomic mapping. A symbolic mapping involves the pairing of unrelated dimensions and, relative to a nomic mapping, requires an additional step in recognition and learning. Using a perceptual identification task, an experiment investigated the hypothesis that nomic mappings are identified more easily than symbolic mappings. It was predicted that the advantage manifests only during the everyday listening experience, and that the initially superior recognition of nomic mappings is equaled by symbolic mappings after extended exposure. The results provided support for the hypotheses. Theoretical implications of the differential recognition of nomic and symbolic mappings are discussed, together with practical applications of nomic relations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ASLIN, R. N., & SMITH, L. B. (1988). Perceptual development. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 435–473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BALLAS, J. A. (1993). Common factors in the identification of an assortment of brief everyday sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(2), 250–267.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • BALLAS, J. A., & HOWARD, J. R. (1987). Interpreting the language of environmental sounds. Environment and Behavior, 19(1), 91–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNARD, W. A., BREEDING, M., & CROSS, H. A. (1984). Object recognition as a function of stimulus characteristics. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22(1), 15–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BRAND, N., & JOLLES, J. (1985). Learning and retrieval rate of words presented auditorily and visually. The Journal of General Psychology, 112(2), 201–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • CARELLO, C., ANDERSON, K. L., & KUNKLER-PECK, A. J. (1998). Perception of object length by sound. Psychological Science, 9, 211–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CHUTE, D. L., & WESTALL, R. F. (1996). Fifth generation research tools: Collaborative development with Powerlaboratory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 28, 311–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • COOK, M. L. (1967). The power law as a special case of Fechner’s law. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 25(1), 51–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DEUTSCH, D. (Ed.). (1999). The psychology of music (2nd ed.). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DOWLING, W. J. (1986). Context effects on melody recognition: Scale-step versus interval representations. Music Perception, 3, 281–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EDWORTHY, J., & STANTON, N. (1995). A user-centered approach to the design and evaluation of auditory warning signals: 1. Methodology. Ergonomics, 38(11), 2262–2280.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FAMILANT, M. E., & DETWEILER, M. C. (1993). Iconic reference: Evolving perspectives and an organizing framework. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 39, 705–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GARNER, W. R., & FELFOLDY, G. L (1970). Integrality of stimulus dimensions in various types of information processing. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 225–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAVER, W. W. (1986). Auditory icons: Using sound in computer interfaces. Human-Computer Interaction, 2, 167–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAVER, W. W. (1989). The SonicFinder: An interface that uses auditory icons. Human-Computer Interaction, 4, 67–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAVER, W. W. (1993a). How do we hear in the world?: Explorations in ecological acoustics. Ecological Psychology, 5(4), 285–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GAVER, W. W. (1993b). What in the world do we hear?: An ecological approach to auditory event perception. Ecological Psychology, 5(1), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GELDARD, F. A. (1962). Fundamentals of psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • GIBSON, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREENE, R. (1988). Stimulus suffix effects in recognition memory. Memory & Cognition, 16(3), 206–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HEINE, W., & GUSKI, R. (1991). Listening: The perception of auditory events? Ecological Psychology, 3(3), 263–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HOWELL, D. C. (1997). Statistical methods for psychology (4th ed.). Belmont: Duxbury Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • JACKO, J. A. (1996). The identifiability of auditory icons for use in educational software for children. Interacting With Computers, 8(2), 121–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • JACKO, J. A., & ROSENTHAL, D. J. (1997). Age-related differences in the mapping of auditory icons to visual icons in computer interfaces for children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 1223–1233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • JENISON, R. L. (1997). On acoustic information for motion. Ecological Psychology, 9, 131–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • JUSCZYK, P. W. (1997). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge: Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • KELLER, P., & STEVENS, C. (2004). Meaning from environmental sounds: Types of signal-referent relations and their effect on recognizing auditory icons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10(1), in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LASS, N. J., EASTHAM, S. K., PARRISH, W. C., SCHERBICK, K. A., & RALPH, D. M. (1982). Listeners’ identification of environmental sounds. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 55, 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LEISER, R. G., AVONS, S. E., & CARR, D. J. (1989). Paralanguage and human-computer interaction. Part 2: comprehension of synthesized vocal segregates. Behaviour & Information Technology, 8(1), 23–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MELARA, R. D. (1989). Dimensional interaction between color and pitch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(1), 69–79.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MELARA, R. D., & MARKS, L. E. (1990). Dimensional interactions in language processing: Investigating directions and levels of crosstalk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 539–554.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAELS, C. E., & CARELLO, C. (1981). Direct perception. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • MICHELL, J. (1986). Measurement scales and statistics: A clash of paradigms. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 398–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MICHELL, J. (1990). An introduction to the logic of psychological measurement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MICHELL, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology: Critical history of a methodological concept. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • MYNATT, E. D. (1997). Transforming graphical interfaces into auditory interfaces for blind users. Human-Computer Interaction, 12, 7–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PANSKY, A., & ZLGOM, D. (1999). Stroop and Garner effects in comparative judgement of numerals: The role of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 39–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • RASCH, R., & PLOMP, R. (1999). The perception of musical tones. D Deutsch (Ed.). The psychology of music (2nd ed.) (pp. 89–112). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • ROSENBLUM, L. D., WUESTEFELD, A. P., & ZNDERSON, K. L. (1996). Auditory reachability: An affordance approach to the perception of sound source distance. Ecological Psychology, 8(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SAVAGE, R., & GOUVIER, W. (1992). Rey auditory-verbal learning test: The effects of age and gender, and norms for delayed recall and story recognition trials. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7(5), 407–414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SHAVELSON, R. J. (1988). Statistical reasoning for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • STEVENS, K., & KELLER, P. (2003). Meaning from environmental sounds: Types of signal-referent relations and their effect on recognizing auditory icons. HFSP Workshop on Music and Language, Macquarie Centre for Cognitive Science, April, http://www.maccs.mq.edu.au/events/2003/hfsp2003/titles.htm

    Google Scholar 

  • STOFFREGEN, T. A., & PITTENGER, J. B. (1995). Human echolocation as a basic form of perception and action. Ecological Psychology, 7(3), 181–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TABACHNICK, B. G., & FIDELL, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • VICENTE, K. J., & BURNS, C. M. (1996). Evidence for direct perception from cognition in the wild. Ecological Psychology, 8(3), 269–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WALKER, B. N., & EHRENSTEIN, A. (2000). Pitch and pitch change interact in auditory displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 15–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • WALKER, B. N., KRAMER, G., & LANE, D. M. (2000). Psychophysical scalings of sonification mappings. Paper presented at the International Conference on Auditory Display, Atlanta, USA, April. Retrieved September 19, 2000, from the World Wide Web: http:www.owlnet.rice.edu/≈walkerb/icad/icad2000/WalkerKramerLane.pdf

    Google Scholar 

  • WARREN, W. H., & VERBRUGGE, R. R. (1984). Auditory perception of breaking and bouncing events: A case study in ecological acoustics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 704–712.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sean W. Coward.

Additional information

This research was conducted in the MARCS Auditory Laboratories and supported by a University of Western Sydney Summer Research Scholarship awarded to Sean Coward. The study benefited from discussions with members of MARCS. In particular, we thank Peter Keller and Michael Tyler for their contribution of ideas and technical assistance, and Denis Burnham for helpful comments on an earlier draft.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Coward, S.W., Stevens, C.J. Extracting Meaning from Sound: Nomic Mappings, Everyday Listening, and Perceiving Object Size from Frequency. Psychol Rec 54, 349–364 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395478

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395478

Navigation