Skip to main content
Log in

Missed opportunities: Psychological ramifications of counterfactual thought in midlife women

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Counterfactual thinking entails the process of imagining alternatives to reality—what might have been. The present study examines the frequency, content, and emotional and cognitive concomitants of counterfactual thinking about past missed opportunities in midlife women. At age 43, nearly two-thirds of the sample of educated adult women reported having missed certain opportunities at some time in their lives. Most of the counterfactual thoughts concerned missed opportunities for greater challenge in work. Emotional distress at age 33 did not predict later counterfactual thought. Instead, counterfactual thinking at age 43 was associated with concurrent emotional distress. However, acknowledging counterfactual thinking about the past was also associated with envisioning ways to change things for the better in the future. This suggests the possibility that the negative appraisal often entailed in counterfactual thinking may be associated with emotional distress in the short run but with motivational benefits in the long run, at least for middle-aged women.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser?Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1982). Learned helplessness, depression, and the illusion of control.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1114–1126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1982).Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review.Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chin, C. (1993).Any regrets? Let me think about it for a minute, or two, or three…. Regret and rumination. Honors Thesis in Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, F., & Lazarus, R. S. (1979). Coping with the stresses of illness. In G. Stone, F. Cohen, & N. E. Adler (Eds.),Health psychology: A handbook (pp. 217–254). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, C. G., Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., Silver, R. C., & Thompson, S. C. (1995). The undoing of traumatic life events.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 109–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eich, E. (1989). Mood dependent memory for internal versus external events.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 443–445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E. (1963).Childhood and society (2nd ed.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erskine, H. (1973). The polls: Hopes, fears, and regrets.Public Opinion Quarterly, 37, 132–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1980).Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1963). Mourning and melancholia.General psychological theory: Papers on metapsychology. New York: Collier Books. (Original work published 1917.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Gavanski, I., & Wells, G. L. (1989). Counterfactual processing of normal and exceptional events.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 314–325.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleicher, F., Kost, K. A., Baker, S. M., Strathman, A., Richman, S. A., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). The role of counterfactual thinking in judgments of affect.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 284–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, N. (1973).Fact, fiction, and forecast (3d ed.) Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gregory, W. L., Cialdini, R. B., & Carpenter, K. M. (1982). Self-relevant scenarios as mediators of likelihood estimates and compliance: Does imagining make it so?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 89–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampshire, S. (1983).Morality and conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helson, R., Mitchell, V., & Hart, B. (1985). Lives of women who became autonomous. Special issue: Conceptualizing gender in personality theory and research.Journal of Personality, 53, 257–285.

    Google Scholar 

  • Isen, A. M. (1984). Toward understanding the role of affect in cognition. In R. S. Wyer & T. K. Srull (Eds.),Handbook of social cognition (Vol. 13, pp. 179–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. K., & Sherman, S. J. (1990). Constructing and reconstructing the past and the future in the present. In E. T. Higgins, & R. M. Sorentino (Eds),Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 482–526). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives.Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982a). The psychology of preferences.Scientific American, 246, 160–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982b) The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.),Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinnier, R. T., & Metha, A. T. (1989). Regrets and priorities at three stages of life.Counseling and Values, 33, 182–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kripke, S. A. (1980).Naming and necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state- vs. action-orientation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 155–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J. (1987). Regret and elation following action and inaction: Affective responses to positive versus negative outcomes.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 524–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J. (1993).Regret: The persistence of the possible. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J. (1995). Through a glass darkly: Worldviews, counter-factual thought, and emotion. In N. J. Roese & J. M. Olson (Eds.),What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 233–258). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, J., & Manis, J. D. (1992). What might have been: Counterfactual thought concerning personal decisions.British Journal of Psychology, 83, 473–477.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, D. R., Wortman, C. B., & Williams, A. F. (1987). Long-term effects of losing a spouse or child in a motor-vehicle crash.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 218–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, D., Darrow, C. N., Klein, E. B., Levinson, M. H., & McKee, B. (1978).The seasons of a man's life. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewinsohn, P. M., Mischel, W., Chaplin, W., & Barton, R. (1980). Social competence and depression: The role of illusory self-perceptions.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 203–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, D. K. (1973).Counterfactuals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macrae, C. N. (1992). A tale of two curries: Counterfactual thinking and accident-related judgments.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 84–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, K. D., Gavanski, I., Sherman, S. J., & McMullen, M. N. (1993). The mental simulation of better and worse possible worlds.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 87–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves.American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metha, A. T., Kinnier, R. T., & McWhirter, E. H. (1989). A pilot study on the regrets and priorities of women.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 13, 167–174.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., & McFarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, E. (1979).Leave your self alone. Grand Rapids, MI: Zondervan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (1993). Self-esteem and counterfactual thinking.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 199–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (Eds.). (1995).What might have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruvolo, A. P., & Markus, H. R. (1992). Possible selves and performance: The power of self-relevant imagery.Social Cognition, 10, 95–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sears, P. S. (1979). The Terman genetic studies of genius, 1922–1972. In A. H. Passow (Ed.),The gifted and the talented: Their education and development. The 78th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education, Part 1. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, S. J., Skov, R. B., Hervitz, E. F., & Stock, C. B. (1981). The effects of explaining hypothetical future events: From possibility to probability to actuality and beyond.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 142–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimanoff, S. B. (1985). Roles governing the verbal expression of emotions between married couples.The Western Journal of Speech Communication, 49, 147–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, A. J. (1975).Longitudinal prediction from personality to life outcomes in college-educated women. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, A. J. (1978). A longitudinal study of coping styles of self-defining and socially defined women.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46, 1079–1084.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, A. J. (1980). Personality and situation in the prediction of women's life patterns.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 5, 195–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, A. J., & Winter, D. G. (1974). Self-definition and social definition in women.Journal of Personality, 42, 238–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, G. (1985).Pride, shame and guilt: Emotions of self-assessment. New York: Clarendon Press, Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. (1983). Adjustment to threatening events: A theory of cognitive adaptation.American Psychologist, 38, 1161–1173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health.Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., & Fogarty, J. J. (1979). Differential effects of induced mood on retrieval of pleasant and unpleasant memories from episodic memory.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 248–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Taylor, R., & Fogarty, J. J. (1980). Effects of induced elation-depression on the accessibility of memories of happy and unhappy experiences.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 339–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wells, G. L., & Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 161–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolitzer, M. (1990, July 15). Vulnerable lives. A review of A. Barnett,The body and its dangers and other stories. The New York Times Book Review, p. 17.

  • Zung, W. W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale.Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 63–70.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Landman, J., Vandewater, E.A., Stewart, A.J. et al. Missed opportunities: Psychological ramifications of counterfactual thought in midlife women. J Adult Dev 2, 87–97 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02251257

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02251257

Key words

Navigation