Skip to main content
Log in

The picture-superiority effect in category-association tests

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Psychological Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two experiments we examined the picture-superiority effect in the framework of the transfer-appropriate processing approach recently advocated by Roediger, Weldon, and Challis (1989). For the first time conceptual implicit-memory task is used, i.e., category association. In Experiment 1, subjects study a mixed list of pictures and words and then receive either a category-association test or a category-cued recall test, followed by a recognition test. The results show that performance on category-cued recall, recognition, and category-association tests are better when the material is studied in picture format. In Experiment 2, we show that producing a sentence with the material studied (picture or word) eliminates the picture-superiority effect in the implicit test, but does not eliminate picture superiority in the category-cued recall test. These results suggest that conceptually driven processing plays a critical role in category association and explicit tests of memory. The results are discussed in the framework of the transfer-appropriate processing approach to memory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Durso, F. T., & Johnson, M. K. (1980). The effects of orienting tasks on recognition, recall, and modality confusion of pictures and words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 416–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, P., & Schacter, D. L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamann, S. B. (1990). Level-of-processing effects in conceptually driven implicit tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 16, 970–977.

    Google Scholar 

  • Intraub, J., & Nicklos, S. (1985). Levels of processing and picture memory: the physical superiority effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning. Memory and Cognition, 11, 284–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacoby, L. L. (1983). Remembering the data: Analysing interactive processes in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 485–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsner, K., Milech, D., & Stumpfel, V. (1986). Word and Picture identification: Is representational parsimony possible? Memory & Cognition, 14, 398–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Madigan, S. (1983). Picture memory. In J. C. Yuille (Ed.), Imagery, memory and cognition: essays in honor of Allan Paivio (pp. 65–89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L. (1979). Remembering pictures and words: Appearance, significance, and name. In L. S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Levels of processing in human memory (pp. 45–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, D. L., Reed, V. S., & McEvoy, C. L. (1977). Learning to order pictures and words: A model of sensory and semantic encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 485–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolas, S. (1987). La Mémoire implicite et ses rapports, avec le souvenir conscient (Implicit memory and its connections with conscious remembering). Unpublished Master's dissersation, University Pierre Mendes France, Grenoble, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolas, S. (1992). La Mémoire et ses manifestations De son expression implicite à son actualisation explicate (Memory and its manifestations: from implicit expression to explicit actualization). Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University Pierre Mendes France, Grenoble, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolas, S. (1993). Mémoire implicite et mémoire explicate: Eléments introductifs (Implicit memory and explicit memory: An introduction). In J. M. Damon, M. Van Der Linden, S. Nicolas, S. Peretti, & F. Sellal, Mémoire explicate, mémoire implicite et pathologies psychiatriques (Explicit memory, implicit memory, and psychiatry) (pp. 23–88). Paris: Masson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolas, S. (1994). Reflexions autour du concept de mémoire implicite (Reflexions on the concept of implicit memory). Année Psychologique, 94, 63–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1969). Mental imagery in associative learning and memory. Psychological Review, 76, 241–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1983). The empirical case for dual coding. In J. C. Yuille (Ed.), Imagery, memory and cognition: Essays in honor of Allan Paivio (pp. 307–332). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representation: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potter, M. C., & Faulconer, B. A. (1975). Time to understand pictures and words. Nature, 253, 437–438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappold, V. A., & Hashtroudi, S. (1991). Does organization improve priming? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 103–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Blaxton, T. (1987). Retrieval modes produce dissociations in memory for surface information. In D. S. Gorfein & R. R. Hoffman (Eds.), Memory and cognitive processes: The Ebbinghaus Centenial Conference (pp. 349–379). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & McDermott, K. B. (1993). Implicit memory in normal human subjects. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neutropsychology (vol. 8, pp. 63–131). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., & Weldon, M. S. (1987). Reversing the picture superiority effect. In M. A. McDaniel & M. Pressley (Eds.), Imagery, and related mnemonic processes: Theories, individual differences, and applications (pp. 151–174). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., & Challis, B. H. (1989). Explaining dissociations between implicit and explicit measures of retention: A processing account. In H. L. Roediger & F. T. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving (pp. 3–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Weldon, M. S., Stadler, M. L., & Riegler, G. L. (1992). Direct comparison of two implicit memory tests: Word fragment and word stem completion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 1251–1269.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, S. J., & Cofer, C. N. (1960). The effect of recency and recall on word association. American Psychologist, 15, 451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L., Bowers, J., & Booker, J. (1989). Intention, awareness, and implicit memory: The retrieval criterion. In S. Lewandowsky, J. C. Dunn, & K. Kirsner (Eds.), Implicit memory: Theoretical issues. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, S. R. (1991). Can we have a distinctive theory of memory? Memory and Cognition, 19, 523–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Branscombe, N. R. (1988). Category accessibility as implicit memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 490–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, M. C., & Magee, L. E. (1980). Tracing the time course of picture-word processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 373–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinivas, K., & Roediger, H. L. (1990). Classifying implicit memory tests: Category association and anagram solution. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 389–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S. (1991). Mechanisms underlying priming on perceptual tests. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17, 526–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S. (1993). The time course of perceptual and conceptual contributions to word fragment completion priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19, 1010–1023.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., & Jackson-Barrett, J. L. (1993). Why do pictures produce priming on the word-fragment completion test? A study of encoding and retrieval factors. Memory and Cognition, 21, 519–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., & Roediger, H. L. (1987). Altering retrieval demands reverses the picture superiority effect. Memory and Cognition, 15, 269–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weldon, M. S., Roediger, H. L., & Challis, B. H. (1989). The properties of retrieval cues constrain the picture superiority effect. Memory and Cognition, 17, 95–105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winnick, W. A., & Daniel, S. A. (1970). Two kinds of responses priming in tachistoscopic recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 84, 74–81.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Nicolas, S. The picture-superiority effect in category-association tests. Psychol. Res 58, 218–224 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419636

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00419636

Keywords

Navigation