Skip to main content

Hands Shaping Communication: From Gestures to Signs

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Hand

Part of the book series: Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics ((SAPERE,volume 38))

Abstract

The focus of this contribution is on the importance of handshapes and is based on data from several studies coming from different perspectives and research traditions. First, we will analyse the emergence of handshapes in infant prehension and fine motor development. Then, we will consider how and to what extent handshapes have played a relevant role in research on gestures in children. Finally, we will describe different trends in the linguistic analyses of handshapes in child and adult uses of sign languages. Bringing these perspectives together for the first time in a single paper provides a better general understanding of the relevant role of the human hand in shaping communication.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The hand is commonly divided into different areas that take their names from the nerves that innervate them: radial, median and ulnar. See also Fig. 2 for an approximate mapping of these areas. Proximal areas are areas closer to the palm, while distal areas are closer to the digits.

  2. 2.

    The concept of “affordance” was initially introduced by Gibson (1979) to indicate invariant features in the environment that offer themselves to an organism supporting or “affording” perceptual or sensory-motor exploitation. For example, a mug may have multiple affordances with a handle that supports grasping and a hollow part that allows carrying liquids.

  3. 3.

    Speech Acts Theory was initially formulated by the philosopher John Austin in a famous series of lectures collected in How to Do things with Words (Austin 1962). Austin showed how, in the pragmatics of communication, sentences can be analysed not only in terms of information conveyed, but as bringing forth three types of actions: the act of saying something (i.e. the locutionary act); what one wishes to communicate in saying it (i.e. the illocutionary act); what causal consequences one wishes to bring about by saying it (i.e. the perlocutionary act). This distinction was later taken up and extended by John Searle who suggested that every statement can be analysed according to its propositional content (i.e. locutionary act) and performative content (i.e. illocutionary act) (Searle 1969; see also Sparaci 2010 for a short review of the relation between Austin’s theory and Bruner’s work).

  4. 4.

    Full-blown pointing is usually defined as a gesture in which “the index finger and arm are extended in the direction of the interesting object, whereas the remaining fingers are curled under the hand, with the thumb held down and to the side” (Butterworth 2003, p. 9).

  5. 5.

    The McArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventories (CDI) are parent questionnaires assessing action and gesture production, word comprehension, word production and early grammatical development between 8- and 36-months (Fenson et al. 2007). They are valid and reliable measures that have been adapted to a number of languages and cultures other than English and used across the world in children with both typical and atypical development. Further information may be fond on the following website: http://mb-cdi.stanford.edu/index.html.

  6. 6.

    For a full description of representational techniques see also Marentette et al. 2016.

  7. 7.

    In the present chapter, all gestures and signs will be reported in capital letters extending to gestures a convention usually employed in sign language studies for signs.

  8. 8.

    All handshapes presented in this paper are represented using the annotation system described in Eccarius and Brentari 2008.

References

  • Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child’s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 468–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ann, J. (1996). On the relation between ease of articulation and frequency of occurrence of handshapes in two sign languages. Lingua, 98, 19–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arbib, M. A., Gasser, B., & Barrès, V. (2014). Language is handy but is it embodied? Neuropsychologia, 55, 57–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Austin, J. L. (1962). How to do things with words. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterley of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205–226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month- olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 359–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernardis, P., Bello, A., Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S., & Gentilucci, M. (2008). Manual actions affect vocalizations in infants. Experimental Brain Research, 184, 599–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Relation between early motor delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for autism. Infant Behavior & Development, 35, 838–846.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boria, S., Fabbri-Destro, M., Cattaneo, L., Sparaci, L., Sinigaglia, C., Santelli, E., et al. (2009). Intention understanding and autism. PLoSOne, 4(5), e5596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyatzis, C. J., & Watson, M. W. (1993). Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64, 729–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyes Braem, P. (1981). Features of the handshape in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyes Braem, P. (1994). Acquisition of handshape in American Sign Language: A preliminary analysis. In V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 107–127). Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyes Braem & Bram. (2000). A pilot study of the expressive gestures used by classical orchestra conductors emmorey. In N. Mahwah (Ed.), The Signs of Language Revisited, Karen & Lane, Harlan. Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyes Braem, P., Pizzuto, E., Volterra, V. (2002). The interpretation of signs by (Hearing and Deaf) members of different cultures. In R. Schulmeister, H. Reinitzer (Eds.), Progress in sign language research. In Honor of Siegmund Prillwitz (pp. 187–219). Hamburg: Signum-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brentari, D., Di Renzo, A., Keane, J., & Volterra, V. (2014). Cognitive, cultural and linguistic sources of a handshape distinction expressing agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7(1), 95–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1974). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language, 2, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to language—A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(3), 255–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, C. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture, pp. 235–257). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, G., & Hopkins, B. (1988). Hand-mouth coordination in the new-born baby. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (2005). From actions to language through gestures. Gesture, 5(1/2), 155–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V., & Graziano, M. (2011). Learning to use gesture in narratives: Developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures (pp. 189–200). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capirci, O., Iverson, J., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Gesture and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 645–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1–143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carter, M. (1981). The acquisition of British Sign Language (BSL: A first analysis), Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words. In V. Volterra, & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 56–67). Berlin/New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V. (2012). Early action and gesture ‘‘Vocabulary’’ and its relation with word comprehension and production. Child Development, 83(2), 526–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23(1), 43–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colletta, J. M. (2004). Le développment de la parole chez l’enfant agé de 6 à 11 ans: corps, language et cognition. Sprimont, Belgique: Mardaga.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conlin, K. E., Mirus, G. R., Mauk, C., & Meier, R. P. (2000). The acquisition of first signs: place, handshape and movement. In C. Chamberlain, J. P. Morford, & R. I. Mayberry (Eds.), Language acquisition by eye (pp. 51–69). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, K., & Dalgleish, M. (1989). The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 894–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Connolly, K., & Elliott, J. (1972). The evolution and ontogeny of hand function. In N. Blurton-Jones (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behaviour (pp. 329–381). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cushing, F. H. (1892). Manual concepts: A study of the influence of hand-usage on culture-growth. The American Anthropologist, 5(4), 289–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutkosky, M. R. (1989). On grasp choice, grasp models, and the design of hands for manufacturing tasks. IEEE Transactions on Robotics and Automation, 5(3), 269–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vries, J. I. P., Visser, G. H. A., & Prechtl, H. F. R. (1982). The emergence of fetal behavior. I Qualitative aspects. Early Human Development, 7, 301–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. W.W. Norton: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dick, A. S., Overton, W. F., & Kovacs, S. L. (2005). The development of symbolic coordination: Representation of hand-as-hands, executive function, and theory of mind. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6, 133–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eccarius, P., & Brentari, D. (2008). Handshape coding made easier: A theoretically based notation for phonological transcription. Sign Language & Linguistics, 11(1), 69–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). The MacArthur communicative development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. E. (1985). Manual actions of nine- to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interactions with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–1279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F. & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and requesting in the second year. Journal of Child Language, 23(20), 307–336.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gentilucci, M., Dalla Volta, R., & Giannelli, C. (2008). When the hands speak. Journal of Physiology—Paris, 102, 21–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geraci, C. (2012). Language policy and planning: The case of Italian sign language. Sign Language Studies, 12(4), 494–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesell, A., & Ilg, F. L. (1937). Feeding behavior of infants. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2017). Using our hands to change our minds. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 8(1–2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Morford, M. (1990). Gesture in early child language. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 249–262). Berlin/New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grover, L. (1988). Comprehension of the pointing gesture in human infants. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Southampton, England: University of Southampton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gullberg, M., de Boot, K., & Volterra, V. (2008). Gestures and some key issues in the study of language development. Gesture, 8(2), 149–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gurney, D. J., Pine, K. J., & Wiseman, R. (2013). The gestural misinformation effect: Skewing eyewitness testimony through gesture. The American journal of psychology, 126(3), 301–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halverson, H. M. (1943). The development of prehension in infants. In R. G. Barker, J. S. Kounin, & Wright, H. F. (Eds.), Child development and behaviour, London: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (2010). Developing language in a developing body: the relationship between motor development and language development. Journal of Child Language, 37(2), 229–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain. The dynamic emergence of speech and gestures. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 19–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karnopp, L. B. (2002). Phonology acquisition in Brazilian Sign Language. In G. Morgan & B. Woll (Eds.), Directions in sign language acquisition (pp. 29–53). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kisch, D. (1996). Adapting the environment instead of oneself. Adaptive Behavior, 4(3/4), 415–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leung, E. H., & Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology, 17, 215–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libertus, K., Sheperd, K. A., Ross, S. W., & Landa, R. L. (2014). Limited fine motor and grasping skills in 6-month-old infants at high risk for autism. Child Development, 85(6), 2218–2231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lifter, K., & Bloom, L. (1989). Object knowledge and the emergence of language. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 395–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind. A theory of material engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A., & Volterra, V. (2016). Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-Speaking Canadian 2-year-olds. Child Development, 87(3), 944–961.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masataka, N. (2003). From index-finger extension to index-finger pointing: Ontogenesis of pointing in preverbal infants. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 69–109). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarty, M. E., Clifton, R. K., & Collard, R. R. (2001). The beginnings of tool use by infants and toddlers. Infancy, 2(2), 233–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McIntire, M. (1977). The acquisition of American Sign Language hand configurations. Sign Language Studies, 16, 247–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex, 48, 788–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, G., Barrett-Jones, S., & Stoneham, H. (2007). The first signs of language: Phonological development in British Sign Language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morissette, P., Ricard, M., & Decarie, T. G. (1995). Joint visual attention and pointing in infancy—A longitudinal study of comprehension. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 163–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Napier, J. R. (1956). The prehensile movements of the human hand. Journal of Bone and Joint, 38-B(4), 902–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Napier, J. R. (1980). Hands. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Science Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K. M. (1986). Constraints on the development of coordination. In M. G. Wade & H. T. A. Whiting (Eds.), Motor development in children: Aspects of coordination and control (pp. 341–360). Boston: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K. M., Scully, D. M., McDonald, P. V., & Baillargeon, R. (1990). Task constraints and infant grip configurations. Developmental Psychobiology, 22(8), 817–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Reilly, A. W. (1995). Using representations: Comprehension and production of actions with hand-as-hands. Child Development, 66, 999–1010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. F., & Jackson, J. P. (1973). The representation of hand-as-hands in action sequences: A developmental study. Child Development, 44, 309–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A baby siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perniss, P., & Vigliocco, G. (2014). The bridge of iconicity: From a world of experience to the experience of language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 369(1651), 20130300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pettenati, P., Sekine, K., Congestri’, E., & Volterra, V. (2012). A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 36(2), 149–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S., & Volterra, V. (2009). Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 887–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pietrandrea, P. (1997). I dizionari della LIS: analisi qualitative e quantitative. In M. C. Caselli & S. Corazza (Eds.), LIS. Studi, esperienze e ricerche sulla lingua dei segni in Italia (pp. 255–259). Pisa: Edizioni del Cerro.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietrandrea, P. & Russo, T. (2007). Diagrammatic and Imagic Hypoicons in signed and verbal languages. In E. Pizzuto, P. Pietrandrea, R. Simone (Eds.), Verbal and signed languages. Comparing structures, constructs and methodologies (pp. 35–56). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pine, K. J., Lufkin, N., & Messer, D. (2004). More gestures than answers: Children learning about balance. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1059–1067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzuto, E., Cameracanna, E., Corazza, S., & Volterra, V. (1995). Terms for spatio-temporal relations in Italian Sign Language (LIS): What they can tell us about iconicity in sign and speech. In R. Simone (Ed.), Iconicity in language (pp. 237–256). New York-Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pizzuto, E., & Corazza, S. (1996). Noun morphology in Italian Sign language (LIS). Lingua, 98, 169–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radutzky, E. (1992). Dizionario bilingue elementare della Lingua Italiana dei Segni. Rome: Ediziona Kappa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J. (2009). What are infant siblings teaching us about autism in infancy? Autism Research, 2, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Russo Cardona, T., & Volterra, V. (2007). Le lingue dei segni. Storia e Semiotica. Roma: Carocci.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sparaci, L. (2010). Discourse and action: analyzing the possibility of a structural similarity. In M. D’Agostino, G. Giorello, F. Laudisa, T. Pievani, & C. Sinigaglia (Eds.), SILFS—New essays in logic and philosophy of science (pp. 493–504). London: College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparaci, L., Northurp, J., Capirci, O. & Iverson, J. M. (forthcoming) From grasping tools to grasping words in infants at high-risk for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sparaci, L., Stefanini, S., D’Elia, L., Vicari, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (2014). What and why understanding in autism spectrum disorders and Williams syndrome: Similarities and differences. Autism Research, 7(4), 421–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sparaci, L., Stefanini, S., Marotta, L., Vicari, S., & Rizzolatti, G. (2012). Understanding motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 50(7), 1639–1649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stefanini, S., Bello, A., Volterra, V., & Carlier, M. (2008). Types of prehension in children with Williams-Beuren syndrome: A pilot study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(3), 358–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Studies in Linguistics, Occasional Papers 8. Surgery, 38B, 902–913.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton-Spence, R., & Woll, B. (1999). The linguistics of British Sign language: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Twitchell, T. (1965). The automatic grasping responses of infants. Neuropsychologia, 3, 247–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twitchell, T. (1970). Reflex mechanisms and the development of prehension. In K. Connolly (Ed.), Mechanisms of motor skill development (pp. 25–45). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vermeerbergen, M., & Demey, E. (2007). Sign + Gesture = Speech + Gesture? Comparing aspects of simultaneity in Flemish Sign Language to instances of concurrent speech and gesture. In M. Vermeerbergen, L. Leeson, & O. Crasborn (Eds.), Simultaneity in signed languages: Form and function (pp. 257–282). Amsterdam: Johns Bejamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Visalberghi, E., & Tomasello, M. (1989). Primate causal understanding in the physical and psychological domains. Behavioural Processes, 42, 189–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Volterra, V. (Ed.). (1987). La Lingua Italiana dei Segni La comunicazione visivo-gestuale dei sordi. Bologna: Il Mulino. (Nuova Edizione 2004).

    Google Scholar 

  • Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L. (2017). Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language Interaction and Acquisition, 8,13–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Tetzchner, S. (1984). First signs acquired by a Norwegian deaf child with hearing parents. Sign Language Studies, 44, 225–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, F. (1998). The hand. New York: Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zinober, B., & Martlew, M. (1985). Developmental changes in four types of gesture in relation to acts of vocalizations from 10 to 21 months. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 293–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This chapter is a part of a project that has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 660468 to LS. Portions of this work were inspired by thought provoking topics discussed during the ABLE Workshop “From Tools and Gestures to the Language-Ready Brain”, Atlanta, GA, April 2010. Authors wish to thank two anonymous reviewers for useful comments on a previous version of this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura Sparaci .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sparaci, L., Volterra, V. (2017). Hands Shaping Communication: From Gestures to Signs. In: Bertolaso, M., Di Stefano, N. (eds) The Hand. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol 38. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-66881-9_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics