Keywords

1 Introduction

Decision Support Tools (DST), defined in their widest sense, exist to support and where possible, improve, decision making. Many real life decisions and particularly those in disciplines such as health, agriculture and financial management are highly complex, multivariate and are taken under conditions of risk and uncertainty. Taking the wrong, or sub-optimal decision may be inconvenient, financially costly or in extreme cases have long lasting health implications. It is particularly important therefore that the information displayed to the decision maker is as clear as it can be. Many modern DST make excellent use of mathematical models to support decision makers in evaluating the implications of choice decisions. The experience of the user in relation to the format of the output from these models may however have an impact on the quality of the decision made.

2 Literature Review

While experts within the field may be familiar with model outputs those called upon to make decisions based on them may not be. In medicine for example, patients with a range of educational backgrounds are becoming increasingly involved in the clinical decision process [1].

Images and graphical representations can reduce cognitive load and increase the efficient processing of information [2] making data more accessible. The nature of the graph employed also has a determining impact on the outcome. Elting et al. [3] demonstrated that in deciding whether or not to continue clinical trials physicians were influenced more by the graphical format than the data.

In an early approach to the type of systematic research that some more recent authors call for [4] Ibrekk and Morgan [5] compared a range of graph types in supporting the decisions of non-experts in conditions of uncertainty. They concluded the best representation depended on the task and hypothesised that the best option might be a combination of cumulative probability density and probability density graphs. The current study tests their hypothesis and then goes on to explore the impact of display type in a more complex, multivariate decision context.

3 Study 1

Methodology

Forty-two non-expert participants (22 males, 20 females) randomly assigned to display conditions were shown graphical displays of meteorological data and asked to judge the likelihood of snowfall and advisability of travel. Six conditions were tested, the five most successful graphical types from the original Ibrekk and Morgan study (bar/column, pie, horizontal shaded probability density, traditional probability density, cumulative probability) and the combination graph Ibrekk and Morgan proposed but did not test (Fig. 1 and Table 1).

Fig. 1.
figure 1

Graph types tested in study 1

Table 1. Percentage correct responses to questions requiring respondents best estimate, estimation of likelihood of more than given amount or probability between two amounts.

Results

Participants did well estimating the amount of snow using the pie chart, shaded graph, probability density graph, and the combined cumulative probability and probability density graph. Chi-square analysis showed a significant difference in the best estimates among the different types of graphs (χ = 45.9, d.f. = 5, p < .001). There were also significant differences among the graphs for the responses to the probability of having more than a certain amount of snow (χ = 39.4, d.f. = 5, p < .001 and the probability of the amount of snow being between two amounts (χ = 33.7, d.f. = 5, p < .001).

4 Study 2

Methodology

This study explored the performance of bar, column and line graphs within more complex symbolic decision contexts, with discrete decision choices. A total of six scenarios were developed, three medical and three environmental. Participants were asked to imagine that they were the decision maker in each. Three types of decision question were asked within each scenario: What, How Much and When. Each of the 30 participants (16 males, 14 females) saw each type of graph under two different scenarios: the order of the graphs and the questions were randomized (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2.
figure 2

examples of the graphical display types used in study 2 (Color figure online)

As there is often no ‘right’ answer in these decision contexts the measure was the individuals confidence in their decision. In line with the previous findings it was hypothesised that a difference between formats would be found.

Results

A Chi-square analysis of the data collapsed over scenarios revealed no significant differences for responses among the graph types. Participants were equally comfortable with their decisions irrespective of display. Most selected the most favourable outcome (Table 2).

Table 2. Showing mean scores for confidence for the representation types

A post-hoc examination of the two types of scenario, environmental and medical did reveal some interesting trends. The data suggested an interaction between graph and scenario type, with a greater degree of confidence being expressed in the environmental scenarios and where a column display was used (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.
figure 3

Showing confidence means for graph types

5 Discussion and Conclusion

The first study, by replicating and extending Ibrekk and Morgan’s original study provides further support for the premise that some formats are better than others in supporting some types of decision task. The second study provides some limited additional evidence that the value of a particular representation is dependent on the nature of the data, and the decision task.

It is possible that a ceiling effect may have masked differences between representation formats in the second study. Cognitive load could have been reduced by the participant’s emotional engagement with the task, each person having some existing belief or experience which provided a cognitive short cut. However no objective measure of the tasks decision complexity was available and this makes it difficult to clearly interpret the results. Future work could explore this possibility.

These studies, particularly the first, support the work of others in identifying differences between graphical layouts in decision quality and decision confidence and underline the impact of decision task on the relative performance of graphical type.