Skip to main content

Towards Behavioral Objects: A Twofold Approach for a System of Notation to Design and Implement Behaviors in Non-anthropomorphic Robotic Artifacts

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Dance Notations and Robot Motion

Part of the book series: Springer Tracts in Advanced Robotics ((STAR,volume 111))

Abstract

Among robots, non-anthropomorphic robotic artifacts are in an interesting position: the fact that they do not resemble living beings, yet impart a sense of agency through the way they move, motivates to consider motion as a source of expressivity in itself, independently of any morphological cues. This problematic is considered in parallel to the question of movement notation and the different levels of abstraction that one may consider when reflecting on movement and its relation to a spatial, temporal and social context. This is through a twofold perspective, drawing on both dance notation and cognitive psychology, that we consider the question of movement notation, and its relation to expressive gestures and psychological attributes. To progress in the direction of a system of notation that could integrate the qualitative, relational, and behavioral aspects of movement, we propose different typologies and a model of constraints to analyze, conceive and implement behaviors in robotic artifacts.

This publication is part of a research program (“The Behavior of Things”) that is supported, through the Labex Arts-H2H, by Investissements d’Avenir (ANR-10-LABX-80-01) of The French National Research Agency (ANR).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We call “behavioral objects” the moving artifacts that a human observer perceives as acting in a meaningful way. Those artifacts need not necessarily be robotic, although their “behavioral” potential is of course reinforced by their ability to react to certain events. Cf. [4].

  2. 2.

    For example: “The Misbehavior of Animated Objects” Workshop, 8th International Conference on Tangible, Embedded and Embodied Interaction (TEI 2014). February 2014, Munich. URL, July 9, 2014: http://www.tei-conf.org/14/studios.php#s9.

  3. 3.

    The MisB Toolkit developed by the EnsadLab/Reflective Interaction team, under the direction of Samuel Bianchini, by Didier Bouchon, Colin Bouvry, Cécile Bucher, Martin Gautron, Benoît Verjat, and Alexandre Saunier, in the context of the project The Behavior of Things, with the support of Labex Arts-H2H and the Bettencourt Schueller Foundation. For more information, URL, July 9, 2014: http://diip.ensadlab.fr/fr/projets/article/the-misb-kit.

  4. 4.

    See for instance project LOL (Laban On Lisp) by Fred Voisin, from Myriam Gourfink’s research in choreography. Cf.: http://www.fredvoisin.com/spip.php?article164 et Frédéric Voisin, LOL: Un environnement expérimental de composition chorégraphique, in Ec/cart, vol. 2, Eric Sadin ed., Dif’Pop, Paris, 2000.

  5. 5.

    The literature on APA is vast, but among the first to have had the intuition and then to have conducted experimental research were: N.A. Bernstein, The coordination and regulation of movements, Oxford, Pergamon, 1967; S.M. Bouisset, M. Zattara, “A sequence of postural movements precedes voluntary movement”, Neuroscience Letter, 1981, n. 22 pp. 263–270.

  6. 6.

    The experiment carried out in the framework of the TechLab workshop of the Dance Monaco Forum (by a team that included, among others, H. Godard, A. Menicacci, E. Quinz, the research team in IRCAM directed by F. Bevilacqua, and the research scientist in behavioral neuroscience I. Viaud-Delmon, CNRS) led to an interesting discovery. By using optical fiber flexion sensors to measure the lateralized movements of two dancers, in particular making repeated arm movements to the right and left, it was possible to determine that the movements involved a whole series of adjustments that were not bilaterally symmetrical as we might have imagined. That indicates that the geometric kinesphere is only a theoretical premise and that when it is experienced through the contingency of a gesture it is dented and asymmetrical. The perceived space is not homogeneous and it contains variable directions and intensities. Cf. A. Menicacci, E. Quinz, “Etendre la perception? Biofeedback et transfert intermodaux en danse”, in “Scientifiquement danse”, Nouvelles de danse 53, Brussels, 2005, pp. 76–96.

  7. 7.

    See the research projects led by Antonio Camurri (Università di Genova, Italy). Starting from the mapping of «emotional indexes» embodied in the body movement, Camurri has developped since 1999 the software EyesWeb and EyesWeb XMI Gesture and Social Processing Software Library. See Camurri [7, 8], et A. Camurri, B. Mazzarino, G. Volpe (2004) Analysis of Expressive Gesture: The EyesWeb Expressive Gesture Processing Library, in A. Camurri, G. Volpe (eds.), Gesture-based Communication in Human-Computer Interaction, LNAI 2915, pp. 460–467, Springer Verlag.

  8. 8.

    The “gestosphere” or “sphere of the gesture” is proposed by Hubert Godard as the ensemble of connections (physical, perceptive, affective, and symbolic) that relate people to their environment. In contrast to Laban, Godard insists on the way the “dynamosphere” is psychologically and symbolically experienced by a person. We could therefore advance that the Godardian gestosphere is based on the dynamosphere, but goes beyond it from the viewpoint of meaning, since it immediately takes into account the psychological experience of the person. Cf. L. Louppe, Poétique de la danse contemporaine, Brussels, Contredanse, 1997, pp. 68–70.

References

  1. J.A. Baird, D.A. Baldwin, Making sense of human behavior: action parsing and intentional inference, in Intentions and Intentionality: Foundations of Social Cognition, eds. by B. Malle, L.J. Moses, D. Baldwin (MIT Press, 2001), pp. 193–206

    Google Scholar 

  2. S. Baron-Cohen, How to build a baby that can read minds: cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Cah. de Psychol. Cogn. Current Psychol. Cogn. 13, 1–40 (1994)

    Google Scholar 

  3. C. Bartneck, T. Kanda, O. Mubin, A. Al Mahmud, Does the design of a robot influence its animacy and perceived intelligence? Int. J. Social Robot. 1(2), 195–204 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  4. S. Bianchini, R. Bourganel, E. Quinz, F. Levillain, E. Zibetti, (Mis)behavioral objects: empowerment of users vs empowerment of objects, in Empowering Users through Design. Interdisciplinary Studies and Combined Approaches for Technological Products and Services, ed. by D. Bihanic (Springer, Berlin, 2015)

    Google Scholar 

  5. C. Breazeal, A. Brooks, M. Hancher, J. Strickon, C. Kidd, J. McBean, D. Stiehl, Public anemone: an organic robot creature, in ACM SIGGRAPH 2002 Conference Abstracts and Applications. SIGGRAPH ’02 (San Antonio, Texas, 21–26 July 2002), pp. 76–76

    Google Scholar 

  6. C. Breazeal, A. Brooks, J. Gray, M. Hancher, J. McBean, D. Stiehl, J. Strickon, Interactive robot theatre. Commun. ACM 46(7), 76–85 (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  7. A. Camurri, S. Hashimoto, M. Ricchetti, R. Trocca, K. Suzuki, G. Volpe, EyesWeb—toward gesture and affect recognition in interactive dance and music systems. Comput. Music J. 24(1), 57–69 (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  8. A. Camurri, EyesWeb, in La scena digitale, nuovi media per la danza, eds. by A. Menicacci, E. Quinz (Venezia Marsilio, 2000)

    Google Scholar 

  9. A. Camurri, G. Volpe (eds.), Gesture-Based Communication in Human-Computer Interaction. LNAI 2915. (Springer, Berlin, 2004)

    Google Scholar 

  10. G. Castellano, A. Camurri, M. Mortillaro, K. Scherer, G. Volpe, Espressive gesture and music: analysis of emotional behaviour in music performance. Music Percep. 25(6), 103–119 (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  11. M. Chion, Guide des objets sonores (Pierre Schaeffer et la recherche musicale. Paris Buchet/Chastel, INA, 1983)

    Google Scholar 

  12. G. Csibra, G. Gergely, The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Dev. Sci. 1(2), 255–259 (1998)

    Google Scholar 

  13. N. Epley, A. Waytz, J.T. Cacioppo, On seeing human: a three-factor theory of anthropomorphism. Psychol. Rev. 114(4), 864–886 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  14. J. Forlizzi, C. DiSalvo, Service robots in the domestic environment: a study of the roomba vacuum in the home, in Proceedings of the 1st ACM SIGCHI/SIGART Conference on Human-Robot Interaction, HRI’06 (ACM, New York, 2–4 Mar 2006), pp. 258–256

    Google Scholar 

  15. H. Fukuda, K. Ueda, Interaction with a moving object affects one’s perception of its animacy. Int. J. Social Robot. 2(2), 187–193 (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  16. T. Gao, G.E. Newman, B.J. Scholl, The psychophysics of chasing: a case study in the perception of animacy. Cogn. Psychol. 59(2), 154–179 (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  17. S.A Gelman, G.M. Gottfried, Children’s causal explanations of animate and inanimate motion. Child Dev. 67, 1970–1987 (1996)

    Google Scholar 

  18. H. Godard, Le geste et sa perception, in La danse au XX ème siècle, eds. by I. Ginot, M. Michel (Larousse, Paris, 2008), p. 236

    Google Scholar 

  19. N. Goodman, Language of the Arts (Indianapolis, New York, 1968)

    Google Scholar 

  20. N. Goodman, in Of Mind and Other Matters. (Harvard U.P, Cambridge, 1984)

    Google Scholar 

  21. J.K. Hamlin, K. Wynn, P. Bloom, Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450, 557–560 (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  22. F. Heider, The psychology of interpersonal relations (Wiley, New York, 1958)

    Google Scholar 

  23. B. Hendriks, B. Meerbeek, S. Boess, S. Pauws, M. Sonneveld, Robot vacuum cleaner personality and behavior. Int. J. Social Robot. 3(2), 187–195 (2011)

    Google Scholar 

  24. M.L. Hoffman, Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2001)

    Google Scholar 

  25. G. Hoffman, W. Ju, Designing robots with movement in mind. J. Human Robot Interac. 3(1), 89–122 (2014)

    Google Scholar 

  26. M. Karg, A.A. Samadani, R. Gorbet, K. Kuhnlenz, J. Hoey, D. Kulic, Body movements for affective expression: a survey of automatic recognition and generation. Affect. Comput. IEEE Trans. 4(4), 341–359 (2013)

    Google Scholar 

  27. R. Laban, Choreutics, pp. 27–36. Dance Books Limited, London (2001) (1966 orig. ed.)

    Google Scholar 

  28. R. Laban, F.C. Lawrence, Effort Shape (Mc Donald and Evans, London, 1947)

    Google Scholar 

  29. P. Lalitte, Conditions de possibilités d’une rhétorique formelle perçue. Intellectica 1–2, 103–114 (2008)

    Google Scholar 

  30. M. Leman, Embodied Music Cognition and Mediation Technology (The MIT Press, Cambridge Massachusetts, 2007)

    Google Scholar 

  31. F. Levillain, S. Lefort, E. Zibetti, Moving on its own: how do audiences interact with an autonomous moving artwork? in Proceedings of the 33rd Annual ACM Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, Proceeding of CHI 2015 (ACM, 2015), pp. 695–702. http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=2702973

  32. B.F. Malle, Attribution theories: how people make sense of behavior, in Theories in Social Psychology, ed. by D. Chadee (2011), pp. 72–95

    Google Scholar 

  33. A. Menicacci, E. Quinz, La scena digitale, nuovi media per la danza (Marsilio, Venezia, 2000)

    Google Scholar 

  34. A. Menicacci, E. Quinz, Etendre la perception? Biofeedback et transfert intermodaux en danse. in Scientifiquement danse, Nouvelles de danse 53, Bruxelles (2005), pp. 76–96

    Google Scholar 

  35. A. Michotte, La Perception de la Causalité (Publications Universitaires de Louvain, 1954)

    Google Scholar 

  36. D. Newtson, Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 28, 28–38 (1973)

    Google Scholar 

  37. M. Noma, N. Saiwaki, S. Itakura, H. Ishiguro, Composition and evaluation of the humanlike motions of an android, in 2006 6th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots (IEEE, 2006), pp. 163–168

    Google Scholar 

  38. A. Paiva, J. Dias, D. Sobral, R. Aylett, P. Sobreperez, S. Woods, C. Zoll, L.E. Hall, Caring for agents and agents that care: building empathic relations with synthetic agents, in Proceedings of the Third International Joint Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems, vol. 1 IEEE Computer Society, 2004), pp. 194–201

    Google Scholar 

  39. A. Pereira, I. Leite, S. Mascarenhas, C. Martinho, A. Paiva, Using empathy to improve human-robot relationships, in Human-Robot Personal Relationships (Springer, Berlin, 2011), pp. 130–138

    Google Scholar 

  40. R.W. Picard, Affective computing: challenges. Int. J. Hum Comput Stud. 59(1), 55–64 (2003)

    Google Scholar 

  41. D. Premack, The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition 36(1), 1–16 (1990)

    Google Scholar 

  42. S.D. Preston, F. De Waal, Empathy: its ultimate and proximate bases. Behav. Brain Sci. 25(01), 1–20 (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  43. J. Saez-Pons, H. Lehmann, D.S. Syrdal, K. Dautenhahn, Development of the sociability of non-anthropomorphic robot home companions, in 2014 Joint IEEE International Conferences on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-Epirob) (IEEE, 2014), pp. 111–116

    Google Scholar 

  44. B.J. Scholl, P.D. Tremoulet, Perceptual causality and animacy. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 299–309 (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  45. P. Shaeffer, À la recherche d’une musique concrète (Seuil, Paris, 1952)

    Google Scholar 

  46. P. Shaeffer, Traité des objets musicaux (Seuil, Paris, 1966)

    Google Scholar 

  47. P. Tremoulet, J. Feldman, Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception 29(8), 943–952 (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  48. P. Tremoulet, J. Feldman, The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion. Percept. Psychophys. 68(6), 1047 (2006)

    Google Scholar 

  49. F. Voisin, LOL: Un environnement expérimental de composition chorégraphique, Paris, Ec/cart, vol. 2, Eric Sadin ed., Dif’Pop (2000)

    Google Scholar 

  50. J.E. Young, J. Sung, A. Voida, E. Sharlin, T. Igarashi, H.I. Christensen, R.E. Grinter, Evaluating Human-Robot Interaction. Int. J. Social Robot. 3(1), 53–67 (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  51. J.M. Zacks, Using movement and intentions to understand simple events. Cogn. Sci. 28(6), 979–1008 (2004)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Samuel Bianchini .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bianchini, S., Levillain, F., Menicacci, A., Quinz, E., Zibetti, E. (2016). Towards Behavioral Objects: A Twofold Approach for a System of Notation to Design and Implement Behaviors in Non-anthropomorphic Robotic Artifacts. In: Laumond, JP., Abe, N. (eds) Dance Notations and Robot Motion. Springer Tracts in Advanced Robotics, vol 111. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-25739-6_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-25739-6_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-25737-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-25739-6

  • eBook Packages: EngineeringEngineering (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics