Skip to main content

Human biochronology: on the source and functions of ‘musicality’

  • Chapter
Book cover Music that works

Abstract

There appear to be two ways to understand or explain our minds scientifically, an introspective/analytical task not normally demanded of us in the activities of daily life. Theorising, estimating or ‘talking about’ the process of our experience leads to the two ways psychologists distinguish what is ‘real’ and what we ‘imagine’ to be so — how what happens in our heads when we perceive is connected to the actual objects we choose to know, and, especially, to what we are doing with them.

Rhythm With Sympathy: Why All God’s Children Have Music In Their Brains’ (with thanks to Louis Armstrong)

The doctrine which I am maintaining is that the whole concept of materialism only applies to very abstract entities, the products of logical discernment. The concrete enduring entities are organisms, so that the plan of the whole influences the very characters of the various subordinate organisms which enter into it. In the case of an animal, the mental states enter into the plan of the total organism and thus modify the plans of successive subordinate organisms until the ultimate smallest organism, such as electrons, are reached. There are thus two sides to the machinery involved in the development of nature. On the one side there is a given environment with organisms adapting themselves to it… The other side of the evolutionary machinery, the neglected side, is expressed by the word creativeness. The organisms can create their own environment. For this purpose the single organism is almost helpless. The adequate forces require societies of cooperating organisms. But with such cooperation and in proportion to the effort put forward, the environment has a plasticity which alters the whole ethical aspect of evolution.“ (A. N. Whitehead, Science and the Modern World, 1926 Emphasis added)

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aitken KJ, Trevarthen C (1997) Self-other organization in human psychological development. Development and Psychopathology 9, 651–675

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson G (1956) The message “This is play”. In: Schaffer B (ed.) Group processes. New York, Josiah Macy Foundation

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson MC (1975) Mother-infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. In: Aaronson D, Rieber RW (eds.) Developmental psycholinguistics and communication disorders: Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 263, pp. 101–113. New York Academy of Sciences, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Beebe B, Jaffe J, Feldstein S, Mays K, Alson D (1985) Inter-personal timing: the application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinesic interactions. In: Field FM, Fox N (eds.) Social perception in infants, pp. 217–248. Norwood, NJ, Ablex

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein N (1967) Coordination and regulation of movements. New York, Pergamon

    Google Scholar 

  • Blacking J (1969/1995) The value of music in human experience. The 1969 Yearbook of the International Folk Music Council. (Republished in Bohlman P, Nettl B (eds.) (1995) Music, culture and experience: selected papers of John Blacking. Chicago, University of Chicago Press. Chapter One, Expressing human experience through music.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogen JE (1969a) The other side of the brain I: dysgraphia and dyscopia following cerebral commissurotomy. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society 34, 73–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogen JE (1969b) The other side of the brain II: an appositional mind. Bulletin of the Los Angeles Neurological Society 34, 135–162

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu P (1990) The logic of practice. Palo Alto, CA, Stanford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandt PA (2009) Music and how we became human — a view from cognitive semiotics: exploring imaginative hypotheses. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 31–44. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bråten S (ed.) (2007) On being moved: from mirror neurons to empathy. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins

    Google Scholar 

  • Buber M (1958) I and thou (2nd edition). Edinburgh, T. and T. Clark Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (1990) How Monkeys see the world: inside the mind of another species. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2007) Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark A (1999) An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3(9), 345–351

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross I (2005) Music and meaning, ambiguity and evolution. In: Miell D, MacDonald R, Hargreaves D (eds.) Musical Communication, pp. 27–43. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Cross I, Morley I (2009) The evolution of music: theories, definitions and the nature of the evidence. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 61–81. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi M, Csikszentmihalyi IS (eds.) (1988) Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Custodero LA (2009) Intimacy and reciprocity in improvisatory musical performance: pedagogical lessons from adult artists and young children. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 513–530

    Google Scholar 

  • Custodero LA, Johnson-Green EA (2003) Passing the cultural torch: musical experience and musical parenting of infants. Journal of Research in Music Education 51(2), 102–114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body, emotion and the making of consciousness. London, Heinemann

    Google Scholar 

  • Delamont RS, Julu POO, Jamal GA (1999) Periodicity of a noninvasive measure of cardiac vagal tone during non-rapid eye movement sleep in non-sleep-deprived and sleep-deprived normal subjects. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology 16(2), 146–153

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deliège, I (ed.) (1999) Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000. Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake E (1988) What is art for? Seattle and London, University of Washington Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Dissanayake E (2000) Art and intimacy: how the arts began. Seattle and London, University of Washington Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald M (1991) Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, Massachusetts, Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Donaldson M (1992) Human minds: an exploration. London, Allen Lane/Penguin Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckerdal P, Merker B (2009) ‘Music’ and the ‘action song’ in infant development: an interpretation. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 241–262. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman C (2002) The construction of mind and self in an interpretive community. In: Brockheimer J, Wang M, Olson D (eds.) Literacy, Narrative, and Culture, London, Curzon

    Google Scholar 

  • Feldman R (2007) Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing: physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48(3/4), 329–354

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Flohr J, Trevarthen C (2007) Music learning in childhood: early developments of a musical brain and body. In: Gruhn W, Rauscher F (eds.) Neurosciences in music pedagogy, pp. 53–100. Hauppauge NY, Nova Science Publishers

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman WJ (2000) Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In: Turner M, Granic I (eds.) Emotion, development, and self-organisation: dynamic systems approaches to emotional development, pp. 209–235. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V (2003) The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychopathology 36(4), 171–180

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Lakoff G (2005) The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in reason and language. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22, 455–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, Houghton Mifflin

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier M (1999) Expressions of belonging: the effect of acculturation on the rhythm and harmony of mother-infant vocal interaction. In: Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000 (93–122). Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier M (2003) Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cognitive Development 18, 533–554

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gratier M (2008) Grounding in musical interaction: evidence from Jazz performances. In: Imberty M, Gratier M (eds.) Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue: Narrative in music and interaction

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier M, Apter-Danon G (2009) The improvised musicality of belonging: repetition and variation in mother-infant vocal interaction. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 301–327. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier M, Trevarthen C (2008) Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies 15(10–11), 122–158

    Google Scholar 

  • Hadamard J (1945) The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday MAK (1975) Learning how to mean: explorations in the development of language. London, Edward Arnold

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz H (1856-1867) Handbuch der physiologischen Optik. Leipzig: Leopold Voss (English Edition, Helmholtz’s Treatise on physiological optics New York: Dover Publications, 1962)

    Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz H (1863) Die Lehre von den Tonempfindungen als physiologische Grundlage für die Theorie der Musik. Braunschweig: F. Vieweg

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess WR (1954) Diencephalon: autonomic and extrapyramidal functions. Orlando, FL, Grune and Stratton

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson P (2002) The cradle of thought: exploring the origins of thinking. London, Macmillan

    Google Scholar 

  • Hubley P, Trevarthen C (1979) Sharing a task in infancy. In: Uzgiris I (ed.) Social interaction during infancy: new directions for child development, 4, 57–80. San Francisco, Jossey-Bass

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl E (1964) The phenomenology of internal time-consciousness (J.S. Churchill, Trans.). Bloomington, Indiana University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Iacoboni M, Molnar-Szakacs I, Gallese V, Buccino G, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G (2005) Grasping the intentions of others with one’s own mirror neuron system. PLoS Biology, March 2005, Volume 3, Issue 3

    Google Scholar 

  • Imberty M (2005) La Music Creuse le Temps. De Wagner à Boulez: Musique, Psychologie, Psychoanalyse. Paris, L’Harmattan

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaffe J, Beebe B, Felstein S, Crown C, Jasnow MD (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing and social development. Society of Child Development Monographs, Serial No. 265, 66(2). Oxford, Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  • James W (1922) What is an emotion? In: Dunlap K (ed.) The emotions, pp. 11–30. Baltimore, Williams and Wilkins. (Original work puplished 1884)

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • James W (1992) Principles of psychology, Vols. 1 and 2. New York, Dover. (Original work published 1890)

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasnow M, Feldstein S (1986) Adult-like temporal characteristics of mother-infant vocal interactions. Child Developement 57, 754–761

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod M (2006) Motor cognition: what actions tell the self. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk PW, Krumhansl CL (1993) Pitch and rhythmic patterns affecting infants’ sensitivity to musical phrase structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 19(3), 627–640

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krumhansl, CL (2000) Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. Psychological Bulletin 126(1), 159–179

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kugiumutzakis G, Kokkinaki T, Markodimitraki M, Vitalaki E (2005) Emotions in early mimesis. In: Nadel J, Muir D (eds.) Emotional development, pp. 161–182. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1999) Philosophy in the flesh, the embodied mind and its challenges to western thought. New York, Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Lecanuet J-P (1996) Prenatal auditory experience. In: Deliege I, Sloboda J (eds.) Musical beginnings: origins and development of musical competence, pp. 3–34. Oxford, New York, Tokyo, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee DN (2005) Tau in action in development. In: Rieser JJ, Lockman JJ, Nelson CA (eds.) Action, perception and cognition in learning and development, pp. 3–49. Hillsdale, N.J., Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee D, Schögler, B (2009) Tau in musical expression. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 83–104. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie AM (1987) Pretence and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review 94, 412–426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie AM (2005) Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(10), 459–462

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Littleton DF (2002) Music in the time of toddlers. Zero to Three 23(1), 35–40

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald RAR, Miell D, Hargreaves DJ (2002) (eds.) Musical identities. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLean PD (1990) The triune brain in evolution, role in paleocerebral functions. New York, Plenum Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch S (1999) Mother and infants and communicative musicality. In: Deliége I (ed.) Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, Liège, Belgium, European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, pp. 29–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) (2009) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companion ship. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch S, Sharp D, Campbell DM, Campbell AM, Trevarthen C (1997) Measuring the human voice: Analysing pitch, timing, loudness and voice quality in mother/infant communication. Proceedings of the Institute of Acoustics 19(5), 495–500

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews J (2004) The art of infancy. In: Eisner E, Day M (eds.) Handbook of research and policy in art education, pp. 253–258. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazokopaki M, Kugiumutzakis G (2009) Infant rhythms: expressions of musical companionship. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 185–208. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen BS (2001) From molecules to mind. Stress, individual differences, and the social environment. Annals of the New York Academic Sciences 93, 42–49

    Google Scholar 

  • McEwen BS, Stellar E (1993) Stress and the individual. Mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine 18,2093–2101

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merker B (2005) The liabilities of mobility: a selection pressure for the transition to cortex in animal evolution. Consciousness and Cognition 14, 89–114

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merker B (2006) Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30, 63–134

    Google Scholar 

  • Merker B, Cox C (1999) Development of the female great call in Hylobates gabriellae: a case study. Folia Primatologica 70, 97–106

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty M (1962) Phenomenology of perception. London, Routledge & Kegan Paul

    Google Scholar 

  • Miall DS, Dissanayake E (2003) The poetics of babytalk. Human Nature 14(4), 337–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miell D, MacDonald R, Hargreaves DJ (eds.) (2005) Musical Communication. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray L (1980) The sensitivities and expressive capacities of young infants in communication with others. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray L, Trevarthen C (1985) Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In: Field TM, Fox NA (eds.) Social Perception in Infants, pp. 177–197. Norwood, NJ, Ablex

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray L, Cooper PJ (eds.) (1997) Postpartum depression and child development. New York, Guilford Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray L, Kempton C, Woolgar M, Hooper R (1993) Depressed mothers’ speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34(7), 1083–1101

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nadel J, Carchon I, Kervella C, Marcelli D, Réserbat-Plantey D (1999) Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science 2(2), 164–173

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nadel J, Guérini C, Pezé A, Rivet C (1999) The evolving nature of imitation as a format for communication. In: Nadel J, Butterworth G (eds.) Imitation in Infancy, pp. 209–234. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel-Brulfert J, Baudonnière P-M (1982) The social function of reciprocal imitation in 2-year-old peers. International Journal of Behavioural Development 5, 95–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy E, Moinár P (2004) Homo imitans or Homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behaviour and Development 27(1), 54–63

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nordoff P, Robbins C (2007) Creative music therapy: a guide to fostering clinical musicianship. First published 1977, NY, John Day. Revised edition. Gilsum, NH, Barcelona Publishers

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne N (2009a) Music for children in zones of conflict and post-conflict: a psychobiological approach. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 331–356. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne N (2009b) Towards a chronobiology of musical rhythm. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communi cative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 545–564. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp J (1998a) The periconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self. Journal of Consciousness Studies 5, 566–582

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp J (1998b) Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp J, Trevarthen C (2009) The neuroscience of emotion in music. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: Exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 105–146. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Pantin CFA (1968) The relations between the sciences. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek M, Papoušek H (1981) Musical elements in the infant’s vocalization: their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. In: Lipsitt LP, Rovee-Collier CK (eds.) Advances in infancy research Vol. 1, pp. 163–224. Norwood, NJ, Ablex

    Google Scholar 

  • Papoušek H, Jürgens U, Papousek M (1992) Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental aspects. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press/Paris: Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne K (2000) The progressively changing songs of humpback whales: a window on the creative process in a non-human mammal. In: Wallin N, Merker B, Brown S (eds.) Origins of music. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1953) The origins of intelligence in children. London, Routledge and Kegan Paul

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J (1954) The construction of reality in the child. (M. Cook, Trans.) New York, Basic Books

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Pöppel E (2002) Three seconds: a temporal platform for conscious activities. In: Grunwald A, Gutmann M, Neumann-Held EM (eds.) On human nature. Wissenschaftethik und Technikfolgenbeurteilung Bd. 15. Springer Verlag, Berlin Heidelberg, New York, pp. 73–79

    Google Scholar 

  • Pöppel E, Wittmann M (1999) Time in the mind. In: Wilson R, Keil F (eds.) The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, pp. 836–837. Cambridge, Massachusetts, The MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Porges SW (1997) Emotion: an evolutionary by-product of the neural regulation of the autonomic nervous system. In: Carter CS, Lederhendler II, Kirkpatrick B (eds.) The integrative neurobiology of affiliation. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 807, pp. 62–78. New York, New York Academy of Sciences

    Google Scholar 

  • Powers N, Trevarthen C (2009) Voices of shared emotion and meaning: young infants and their mothers in Scotland and Japan. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 209–240. Oxford. Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy V (2003) On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7, 397–402

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy V (2005) Feeling shy and showing-off: self-conscious emotions must regulate self-awareness. In: Nadel J, Muir D (eds.) Emotional development, pp. 183–204. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy V (2008) How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L, Gallese V (2006) Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American 295(5), 30–37

    Google Scholar 

  • Robarts JZ (2009) Supporting the development of mindfulness and meaning: clinical pathways in music therapy with a sexually abused child. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 377–400. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Robb L (1999) Emotional musicality in mother-infant vocal affect, and an acoustic study of postnatal depression. In: Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, pp. 123–151. Liège, European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

  • Roederer JG (1995) The physics and psychophysics of music: an introduction. New York, Springer-Verlag

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff B (2003) The cultural nature of human development. Oxford, OUP

    Google Scholar 

  • Schögler B, Trevarthen C (2007) To sing and dance together. In: Bråten S (ed.) On being moved: from mirror neurons to empathy, pp. 281–302. Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins

    Google Scholar 

  • Schögler BW (1999) Studying temporal co-ordination in Jazz Duets. In: Rhythms, Musical Narrative, and the Origins of Human Communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, pp. 75–92. Liège, European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

  • Sillar KT, Reith CA, McDearmid JR (1998) Development and aminergic neuromodulation of a spinal locomotor network controlling swimming in Xenopus larvae. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 860, pp. 318–332

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith A (1777/1982) Bryce JC; with Dugald Stewart’s account of Adam Smith, edited by Ross IS. General editors, Raphael DD, Skinner AS) Indianapolis, Liberty Fun

    Google Scholar 

  • Smuts B (2001) Encounters with animal minds. In: Thompson E (ed.) Between ourselves: second-person issues in the study of consciousness, pp. 293–309. Charlottesville, VA/Thorverton, UK, Imprint Academic

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry RW (1950) Neuronal basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative Physiology 43, 483–489

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry RW (1952) Neurology and the mind-brain problem. American Scientist 40, 291–312

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperry RW (1982) Some effects of disconnecting the cerebral hemispheres (Nobel lecture). Science 217, 1223–1226

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Standley JM (2002) Music therapy in the NICU: promoting growth and development of premature infants. Zero to Three 23(1), 23–30

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (1992) L’enveloppe prénarrative: Vers une unité fondamentale d’expérience permettant d’explorer la réalité psychique du bébé. Revue Internationale de Psychopathologie 6, 13–63

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (1999) Vitality contours: the temporal contour of feelings as a basic unit for constructing the infant’s social experience. In: Rochat P (ed.) Early social cognition: understanding others in the first months of life, pp. 67–90. Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (2000) The interpersonal world of the infant: a view from psychoanalysis and development psychology. (Originally published in 1985. Paperback Second Edition, with new Introduction) Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN (2004) The present moment. In: Psychotherapy and everyday life. New York, Norton

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern DN, Gibbon J (1980) Temporal expectancies of social behaviours in mother-infant play. In: Thoman E (ed.) Origins of infant social responsiveness, pp. 409–429. New York, Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson E (ed.) (2001) Between ourselves: second-person issues in the study of consciousness. Charlottesville, VA/Thorverton, UK, Imprint Academic, and Journal of Consciousness Studies 8, 5–7

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen N (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford, Clarendon Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trainor LJ (2002) Lullabies and playsongs: why we sing to children. Zero to Three 23(1), 31–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE (1990) The perception of musical patterns by human infants: the provision of similar patterns by their parents. In: Berkley MA, Stebbins WC (eds.) Comparative perception, Vol. 1, Mechanisms, pp. 429–459. New York, Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE, Trainor LJ, Unyk AM (1993) Music and speech processing in the first year of life. Advances in Child Development and Behaviour 24, 1–35

    Google Scholar 

  • Trehub SE, Unyk AM, Trainor LJ (1993) Maternal singing in cross-cultural perspective. Infant Behaviour and Development 16, 285–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1977) Descriptive analyses of infant communication behavior. In: Schaffer HR (ed.) Studies in mother-infant interaction: the Loch Lomond symposium. London, Academic press, pp. 227–270

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1984) Hemispheric specialization. In: Darian-Smith I (ed.) handbook of physiology. Section 1: The nervous system, Vol. III: Sensory processes. Bethesda, Maryland (American physiological society)

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1986) Development of intersubjective motor control in infants. In: Wade MG, Whiting HTA (eds.) Motor development in children: aspects of coordination and control, pp. 209–261. Dordrecht, Martinus Nijhof

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1988) Universal cooperative motives: how infants begin to know language and skills of culture. In: Jahoda G, Lewis IM (eds.) Acquiring culture: ethnographic perspectives on cognitive development, pp. 37–90. London, Croom Helm

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1990) Signs before speech. In: Sebeok TA, Umiker-Sebeok J (eds.) The semiotic web, 1989. Berlin, New York, Amsterdam, Mouton de Gruyter, 689–755

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1993) The function of emotions in early infant communication and development. In: Nadel J, Camaioni L (eds.) New perspectives in early communicative development, pp. 48–81. London, Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1994) Infant semiosis. In: Nöth W (ed.) Origins of semiosis. Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter, 219–252

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1996) Lateral asymmetries in infancy: implications for the development of the hemispheres. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 20(4): 571–586

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1998) The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In: Bråten S (ed.) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, pp. 15–46. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. In: Deliège I (ed.) Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication Musicae scientiae, Special issue, 1999–2000, pp. 157–213. Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2001) The neurobiology of early communication: intersubjective regulations in human brain development. In: Kalverboer AF, Gramsbergen A (eds.) Handbook on brain and behavior in human development, pp. 841–882. Dordrecht, The Netherlands, Kluwer

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2002) Origins of musical identity: evidence from infancy for musical social awareness. In: MacDonald R, Hargreaves D, Miell D (eds.) Musical identities, pp. 21–38. Oxford, Oxford university press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2004) How infants learn how to mean. In: Tokoro M, Steels L (eds.) A learning zone of one’s own. (SONY future of learning series), pp. 37–69. Amsterdam, IOS Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2005) Action and emotion in development of the human self, its sociability and cultural intelligence: why infants have feelings like ours. In: Nadel J, Muir D (eds.) Emotional development, pp. 61–91. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2006a) Wer schreibt die Autobiographie eines Kindes. In: Markowitsch H, Welzer H (eds.) Warum Menschen Sich Erinnern Können. (Fortschritte der Interdisziplinären Gedächtnisforschung), pp. 225–255. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. (Trans. Karoline Tschuggnall)

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C (2006b) First things first: infants make good use of the sympathetic rhythm of imitation, without reason or language. Journal of Child Psychotherapy 31(1), 91–113

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ (1994) Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: intrinsic factors in child mental health. Development and Psychopathology 6, 599–635

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ (2003) Regulation of brain development and age-related changes in infants’ motives: the developmental function of “regressive” periods. In: Heimann M (ed.) Regression periods in human infancy, Mahwah, NJ, Erlbaum, pp. 107–184

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Gratier M (2005) Voix et Musicalité. Nature, Emotion, Relations et culture. In: Castarède M-F, Konopczynski G (eds.) Au Commencement Était la Voix. Ramonville Saint-Agne: Érès, pp. 105–116

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Hubley P (1978) Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock A (ed.) Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language. London, pp. 183–229. New York, San Francisco, Academic press

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Malloch S (2002). Musicality and music before three: human vitality and invention shared with pride. Zero to Three, 23(1), 10–18

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Marwick H (1986) Signs of motivation for speech in infants, and the nature of a mother’s support for development of language. In: Lindblom B, Zetterstrom R (eds.) Precursors of early speech. Basingstoke, Hampshire, Macmillan, pp. 279–308

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Reddy V (2007) Consciousness in infants. In: Velman M, Schneider S (eds.) A companion to consciousness, pp. 41–57. Oxford, Blackwell

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ, Vandekerckhove M, Delafield-Butt J, Nagy E (2006) Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ (eds.) Developmental psychopathology, Developmental neuroscience, Vol. 2, pp. 65–126. Second Edition. New York, Wileys

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick EZ (2005) Why is connection with others so critical? The formation of dyadic states of consciousness: coherence governed selection and the co-creation of meaning out of messy meaning making. In: Nadel J, Muir D (eds.) Emotional development, pp. 293–315. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick EZ, Als H, Adamson L, Wise S, Brazelton TB (1978) The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 17, 1–13

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W (eds.) Organization of memory, pp. 381–403. New York, Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (2002) Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53, 1–25

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner M (1996) The literary mind: the origins of thought and language. New York/Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner F, Pöppel E (1988) Metered poetry, the brain, and time. In: Rentschler I, Herzberger B, Epstein D (eds.) Beauty and the brain. Biological Aspects of Aesthetics, pp. 71–90. Birkhäuser Verlag, Basel

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner R, Ioannides A (2009) Brain, music and musicality: inferences from neuroimaging. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds.) Communicative musicality: exploring the basis of human companionship, pp. 147–181. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • van Rees S, de Leeuw R (1993) Born too early: the Kangaroo method with premature babies. Video by Stichting Lichaamstaal, Scheyvenhofweg 12, 6093 PR, Heythuysen, The Netherlands

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ (1999) Present-time consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 6(2–3), 111–140

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela FJ, Thompson E, Rosch E (1991) The embodied mind. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950) Das Reafferenzprinizp. Die Naturwissenschaften 37, 464–475

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Holst E (1973) The behavioural physiology of animals and man (originally, Zur Verhaltensphysiologie bei Tieren und Menschen). London, Methuen

    Google Scholar 

  • von Holst E, von Saint Paul U (1962) Electrically controlled behaviour. Scientific American, March 1962

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky LS (1967) Play and its role in the mental development of the child. Soviet Psychology 5(3), 6–18. Republished in: Bruner, Jolly and Sylva, Play — its role in development and education, 1985

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallin N (1991) Biomusicology: neurophysiological, neuropsychological, and evolutionary perspectives on the origins and purposes of music. New York, Pendragon Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallin NL, Merker B, Brown S (eds.) (2000) The origins of music. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Watanabe H, Malloch S, Trevarthen C, Horiuchi T, Watanabe T (2006) Workshop 19: communicative musicality and premature infants. Abstract 399, 10th World Congress, World Association of Infant Mental Health, Paris, 8–12 July, 2006

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead AN (1926) Science and the modern world. Lowell lectures, 1925. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead AN (1929) Process and reality: an essay in cosmology. Giffor Lectures 1927–1929. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead AN, Russell B (1910, 1912, 1913) Principia Mathematica, 3 volumes. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittman M, Pöppel E (1999) Temporal mechanisms of the brain as fundamentals of communication — with special reference to music perception and performance. In Rhythms, musical narrative, and the origins of human communication. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, 1999–2000, pp. 13–28. Liège: European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer-Verlag/Wien

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Trevarthen, C. (2009). Human biochronology: on the source and functions of ‘musicality’. In: Haas, R., Brandes, V. (eds) Music that works. Springer, Vienna. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-211-75121-3_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics