Skip to main content

The Evo–Eco Approach to Behaviour Change

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Applied Evolutionary Anthropology

Part of the book series: Advances in the Evolutionary Analysis of Human Behaviour ((AEAHB,volume 1))

Abstract

We introduce a new approach to behaviour change called ‘Evo–Eco’ because of its intellectual roots in evolutionary biology and ecological psychology. This approach is based on the inference that brains evolved to provide adaptive behavioural responses to rapidly changing or complex environmental conditions. From this foundation, we develop a model with three basic components: the environment, which presents some challenge or opportunity to the individual; the brain, which produces potential responses to that challenge; and the body, which engages in interactions with the environment (i.e. produces behaviour) that changes that environment. The behaviours of interest to behaviour-change professionals typically occur in particular settings, which can be seen as a context within which these basic components interact. We report how the approach has been used to develop public health programmes, as well as to make novel predictions about behavioural causes (i.e. placement of new target behaviours within a routine) which have proved to impact on the ability to change a behaviour. The Evo–Eco approach thus deserves to be used by public health workers and others to change behaviour.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abramson, C. (1994). A primer of invertebrate learning: The behavioral perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(4), 547–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R., & Valerie, C. (accepted). Gaining control: The Evolution of Complex Behaviour in Humans. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (2007). Toothbrushing as routine behaviour. International Dental Journal, 57, 364–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (2010b). Types of technology. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 77, 762–782.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (2008). Kinds of behaviour. Biology and Philosophy, 23(3), 317–345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (2013). The anatomy of motivation: An evolutionary ecological approach. Biological Theory, 8, 49–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (in press). Gaining control: Major transitions in the evolution of human behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baars, B. (1997). In the theater of consciousness: The workspace of the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balleine, B. W., & O’Dohety, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control: Corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology: official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 35, 48–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1965). Vicarious processes: A case of no-trial learning. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 1–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G. (1968). Ecological Psychology: Concepts and methods for studying the environment of human behavior. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. G., & Schoggen, P. (1973). Qualities of community life: Methods of measuring environment and behavior applied to an American and an English town. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, G. (1977). Modal action patterns. In G. Barlow & T. Sebeok (Eds.), How animals communicate (pp. 98–134). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, L. (2011). Beyond the brain: How body and environment shape animal and human minds. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Tierney, J. (2012). Willpower: Rediscovering the Greatest Human Strength. London: Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the science of self-reports and finger movements: Whatever happened to actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 396–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biran, A., Schmidt, W.-P., Divya, R., Sanker, K. G., Aunger, R., Mario V., & Curtis, V. (accepted). A randomized, controlled intervention trial of a village-level intervention to promote handwashing with soap in rural Indian households. Lancet Global Health.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P., & Lienard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action-parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 1–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, A. B. (2001). Brain evolution and comparative neuroanatomy. Encyclopedia of the life sciences (pp. 1–8). London: Macmillan Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cairncross, S., Bartram, J., Cumming, O., & Brocklehurst, C. (2010). Hygiene, sanitation, and water: What needs to be done? PLoS Medicine, 7(11), e1000365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlin, J. (2011). Cease fire. Monitor on Psychology, 42(6), 84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherry, M. G., Brown, J. M., Bethell, G. S., Neal, T., & Shaw, N. J. (2012). Features of educational interventions that lead to compliance with hand hygiene in healthcare professionals within a hospital care setting. A BEME systematic review: BEME Guide No. 22. Medical Teacher, 34(6), 406–420.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain body and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, J. (1990). Foundations of social theory. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, R. (2009). Gruesome photos on cigarette packages reduce tobacco use. Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 87(8), 569–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V. (2013). Don’t look, don’t touch, don’t eat: The science behind revulsion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Cousens, S., Mertens, T., Traoré, E., Kanki, B., & Diallo, I. (1993). Structured observations of hygiene in Burkina Faso, validity, variability and utility. Bulletin of the World Health Organisation, 71(1), 23–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Kanki, B., Cousens, S., Diallo, I., Kpozehouen, A., Sangaré, M., & Nikiema, M. (2001). Evidence for behaviour change following a hygiene promotion programme in West Africa. Bulletin of the World Health Organisation, 79(6), 518–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Danquah, L. & Aunger, R. (2009). Planned, motivated and habitual hygiene behaviour: An eleven country review. Health Education Research 24 (4): 655–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, V., Schmidt, W., Luby, S., Florez, R., ToureÌ, O., & Biran, A. (2011). Hygiene: New hopes, new horizons. Lancet Infectious Diseases, 11, 312–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daw, N. D., & Shohamy, D. (2008). The cognitive neuroscience of motivation and learning. Social Cognition, 26(5), 593–620.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daw, N. D., Niv, Y., & Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1704–1711.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron, 69(6), 1204–1215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dayan, P., Niv, Y., Seymour, B., & Daw, D. N. (2006). The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Networks, 19(8), 1153–1160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: The development of behavioral autonomy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 308, 67–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. (2002). The role of learning in the operation of motivational systems. In H. Pashler & R. Gallistel (Eds.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology. (Third Edition) Vol. 3. Learning, motivation, and emotion (pp. 497–534). New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eichenbaum, H. (2000). Declarative memory. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eilam, D., Zor, R., Szechtman, H., & Hermesh, H. (2006). Ritual, stereotypy and compulsive behavior in animals and humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 456–471.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engle, P. L., Black, M. M., Behrman, J. R., Cabral de Mello, M., Gertler, P. J., Kapiriri, L., Young, & M., E. (2007). Strategies to avoid the loss of developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the developing world. Lancet, 369(9557), 229–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: From actions to habits to compulsion. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1481–1489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Falomir, J. M., & Invernizzi, F. (1999). The role of social influence and smoker identity in resistance to smoking cessation. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 58, 73–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., Triandis, H., & Kanfer, F. (2001). Factors influencing behaviour and behaviour change. In A. E. Baum, T. A. Revensen, & J. E. Singer (Eds.), Handbook of health psychology (pp. 3–17). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flay, B. R., Snyder, F., & Petraitis, J. (2009). The theory of triadic influence. In R. J. DiClemente, M. C. Kegler, & R. A. Crosby (Eds.), Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research (2nd ed., pp. 451–510). New York: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleischman, D. S., Webster, G. D., Judah, G., Barra, M., Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (2011). Sensor recorded changes in rates of hand washing with soap in response to the media reports of the H1N1 pandemic in Britain. BMJ Open, 1(2), e000127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeman, W. J. (1999). How the brain makes up its mind. London: Weidenfeld and Nicholson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G. Jr, & Laland, K. N. (2005). Social learning in animals: Empirical studies and theoretical models. BioScience, 55(6), 489–499.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greif, M. L., Nelson, D. G. K., Keil, F. C., & Gutierrez, F. (2006). What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information. Psychological Science, 17(6), 455–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hargreaves, T. (2011). Practice-ing behaviour change: Applying social practice theory to pro-environmental behaviour change. Journal of Consumer Culture, 11(1), 79–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L. (2001). On a folk theory of society: Children, evolution, and mental representations of social groups. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 5, 107–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huey, R., Hertz, P., & Sinervo, B. (2003). Behavioral drive versus behavioral inertia in evolution: A null model approach. American Naturalist, 161, 357–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, G. E. (1957). Concluding remarks. Cold Spring Harbor Symposia on Quantitative Biology, 22, 415–427.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2006). Young children’s conception of the biological world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(4), 177–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind, 19, 188–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, M., & Curtis, V. (2005). Achieving the ‘good life’: Why some people want latrines in rural Benin. Social Science and Medicine, 61, 2446–2459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judah, G., Aunger, R., Curtis, V., Schmidt, W.-P., & Michie, S. (2009). Experimental pre-testing of potential public health interventions: The case of handwashing with soap. American Journal of Pubic Health, 99, S405–S411.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judah, G., Gardner-Sood, B., & Aunger, R. (2012). Forming a flossing habit: An investigation into the psychological determinants of habit formation. British Journal of Health Psychology, 18, 338–353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kotler, P., Roberto, N., & Lee, N. R. (2002). Social marketing: Improving the quality of life (2nd ed.). New York: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krasne, F. B., & Glanzman, D. L. (1995). What we can learn from invertebrate learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 46, 585–624.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R., & Dawkins, R. (1978). Animal signals: Mind reading and manipulation. In :J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behaviour ecology: An evolutionary approach (pp. 380–402). Sunderland: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lally, P., van J., C. H. M., Potts, Henry, W. W., & Wardle, J. (2009). How are habits formed: Modelling habit formation in the real world. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(6), 998&1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorenz, K. (1950). The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns. In J. F. Danielli & R. Brown (Eds.), Physiological mechanisms in animal behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(50), 19751–19756.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1985). Conditioning and associative learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie-Mohr, D., & Smith, W. (2000). Fostering sustainable behaviour: An introduction to community-based social marketing. Gabriola Island: New Society Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D., & Atran, S. (1998). Folk biology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Medin, D., & Atran, S. (2004). The native mind: Biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review, 111 (4), 960–983.

    Google Scholar 

  • Millikan, R. G. (2000). On clear and confused ideas: An essay about substance concepts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mitani, J. C., Call, J., Kappeler, P. M., Palombit, R. A., & Silk, J. B. (2012). The evolution of primate societies. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mittal, B. (1988). Achieving higher seat belt usage: The role of habit in bridging the attitude-behavior gap. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 993–1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moon, J. A. (2004). A handbook of reflective and experiential learning: Theory and practice. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neal, D. T., Wood, W., Labrecque, J. S., & Lally, P. (2012). How do habits guide behavior? Perceived and actual triggers of habits in daily life. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 492–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northcutt, R. G., & Kaas, J. H. (1995). The emergence and evolution of mammalian neocortex. Trends in the Neurosciences, 18, 373–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostlund, S. B., Winterbauer, N. E., & Balleine, B. W. (2009). Evidence of action sequence chunking in goal-directed instrumental conditioning and its dependence on the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8280–8287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, J., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 54–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Owen, N., Humpel, N., Leslie, E., Bauman, A., & Sallis, J. F. (2004). Understanding environmental influences on walking: Review and eesearch agenda. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 27, 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pasupathy, A., & Miller, E. K. (2005). Different time courses of learning-related activity in the prefrontal cortex and striatum. Nature, 433, 873–876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, H. W. (2003). The social norms approach to preventing school and college age substance abuse: A handbook for educators, counselors, and clinicians. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plotkin, H. (1988). Learning and evolution. In H. Plotkin (Ed.), The role of behavior in evolution. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Asarnow, R. F., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Knowlton, B. J. (2005). The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 5356–5364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Potts, R. (2012). Evolution and environmental change in early human prehistory. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41, 151–167.

    Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J. (2000). Folk physics for apes: The chimpanzee’s theory of how the world works. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prochaska, J., & DiClemente, C. (1983). Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: Toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 390–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quartz, S. (2001). Toward a developmental evolutionary psychology: Genes, development, and the evolution of human cognitive architecture. In S. Scher & M. Rauscher (Eds.), Evolutionary psychology: Alternative approaches. Norwell: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rabie, T., & Curtis, V. (2006). Evidence that handwashing prevents respiratory tract infection: A systematic review. Tropical Medicine and International Health, 11(3), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ravenscroft, I. (2010). Folk psychology as a theory. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2010 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (Ed.). http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2010/entries/folkpsych-theory/

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riker, W. H., & Ordeshook, P. C. (1973). An introduction to positive political theory. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, E. (1995). Diffusion of innovations (4th ed.). New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sallis, J. F., Cervero, R. B., Ascher, W., Henderson, K. A., Kraft, M. K., & Kerr, J. (2006). An ecological approach to creating active living communities. Annual Review of Public Health, 27, 297–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans goals and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schoggen, P. (1989). Behaviour settings: A revision and extension of Roger G. Barker’s ecological psychology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: How to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 57, 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, B. E., Lawson, D. W., & Curtis, V. (2007). Hard to handle: Understanding mothers' handwashing behaviour in Ghana. Health Policy and Planning, 22 (4), 216–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serruya, D., & Eilam, D. (1996). Stereotypies, compulsions, and normal behavior in the context of motor routines in the rock hyrax (Procavia capensis). Psychobiology, 24, 235–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science, 183, 482–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (2000). Metarepresentations in evolutionary perspective. In D. Sperber (Ed.), Metarepresentations: A multidisciplinary perspective (pp. 117–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (2003). Adapative behavior and learning (2nd electronic ed). from http://psychandneuro.duke.edu/research/labs/staddon-lab

  • Sterelny, K. (2003). Mind in a hostile world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokols, D. (1992). Establishing and maintaining healthy environments: Toward a social ecology of health promotion. American Psychologist, 47, 6–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stokols, D. (1995). The paradox of environmental psychology. American Psychologist, 50, 821–837.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streidter, G. F. (2005). The principles of brain evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1998). Reinforcement learning: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (2001). Chunking and serially organized behavior in pigeons, monkeys and humans. In R. G. Cook (Ed.), Avian visual cognition. Medford: Comparative Cognition Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1901). Animal intelligence: An eperimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review Monograph Supplement, 2, 1–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychology Review, 55, 189–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1985). How many memory systems are there? American Psychologist, 40, 385–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wcislo, W. (1989). Behavioral environments and evolutionary change. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 20, 137–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinstein, N. D. (1988). The precaution adoption process. Health Psychology, 7, 355–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, R., Walia, A., Hyder, N., Shahab, L., & Michie, S. (2010). Behavior change techniques used by the English Stop Smoking Services and their associations with short-term quit outcomes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 12(7), 742–747.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, R. G. (2005). The impact of inequality: How to make sick societies healthier. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willis, W. D. (1985). The pain system: The neural basis of nociceptive transmission in the mammalian nervous system. Basel: Kragel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, D. S., Hayes, S. C., Biglan, A., & Embry, D. D. (in press). Evolving the future: Toward a Science of Intentional Change. Behavioural and Brain Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Witte, K., & Allen, M. (2000). A meta-analysis of fear appeals: Implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Education & Behavior, 27(5), 591–615.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wunderlich, K., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2012). Mapping value based planning and extensively trained choice in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 786–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyles, J., Kunkel, J., & Wilson, A. (1983). Birds, behavior, and anatomical evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 80, 4394–4397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459–482.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin, H. H., & Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 464–476.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, M. (1988). The metronomic society: Natural rhythms and human timetables. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Adam Biran, Micheal de Barra, Katie Greenland, Sharon Guten, Gaby Judah, Hans-Joachim Mosler, Helen Trevaskis, Robert West, Allan Wicker, two anonymous referees and the editors for contributing to earlier versions of this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Aunger .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Aunger, R., Curtis, V. (2014). The Evo–Eco Approach to Behaviour Change. In: Gibson, M., Lawson, D. (eds) Applied Evolutionary Anthropology. Advances in the Evolutionary Analysis of Human Behaviour, vol 1. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0280-4_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics