Skip to main content

Intentionality and Time in Human Development and Aging: Compensation and Goal Adjustment in Changing Developmental Contexts

  • Chapter
Understanding Human Development

Abstract

Time is both an action resource and a source of meaning in human development over the life span. The fading of residual life time in old age thus poses particular adaptive challenges that call for selective, compensatory, and optimizing activities. Such activities depend not only on the causal implications of aging, but likewise on the practical conclusions that people draw from having a given age and a given position in the life cycle. The present chapter addresses the different ways in which time, as perceived or personalized time, affects the negotiation of gains and losses as well as the construal of meaning in later life. The main theoretical focus is on the adaptive interplay between between self-regulatory and compensatory activities and mechanisms through which personal goals are adjusted to changes in action resources that are linked to both ontogenetic and historical dimensions of time.

Developmental processes begin, unfold, and end in time. In developmental research, the time axis traditionally serves as an ordering device. Age-graded ontogenetic changes as well as age-linked developmental tasks are ordered along a biographical time dimension, whereas cultural changes, which affect members of different age cohorts at different points of their lifecycle, are charted on a historical time dimension. It is commonly assumed that age and time have no intrinsic explanatory power; when moving from description toward explanation, researchers try to identify causal factors that, on biographical and historical dimensions, are linked to the flow of time (such as age-graded changes in bodily maturation or socialization or history-graded changes in political or economic conditions of society).

Human development, however, is linked to time not only by a causal-physical nexus; it is the joint product of causality and intentionality. Intentionality, as it is expressed in life planning and the pursuit of goals over the life course, is intrinsically related to phenomenological, personalized time. In retrospect, personalized time is filled with changes and events that are more or less consistent with the person’s current preferences; in prospect, it constitutes an action space filled with risks, potentialities, and more or less attractive possible selves (Brandtstädter, 2001; Heckhausen, this volume; Roberts & Caspi, this volume).

The importance of time perspectives in action, motivation, and life planning has been recognized for a long time (e.g., Lewin, 1953; Nuttin, 1964). Time, and the coordination of physical and perceived time, is needed not only to pursue goals but also to orchestrate and harmonize our strivings: goals that are logically or practically incompatible when simultaneously pursued may become feasible when arranged in a temporal sequence. Gains and losses emerge in time, and their weight in personal planning and decision making underlies a temporal discounting. Furthermore, time is a compensatory resource: we need time to cope with loss, and even slow runners (or learners) may reach the goal when they are granted enough time. Our projects and life activities gain meaningful valence from intended future goals, and they may lose sense and meaning when these intentional links are disrupted (e.g., Klinger, 1977).

Time is thus both an action resource and a source of meaning. Temporal resources, however, are limited. Most important, our lifetime itself is limited; some of our goals and projects become unfeasible as we move through historical and ontogenetic time. In preventive and compensatory efforts, we invest time to expand our temporal resources or make more efficient use of them (M. Baltes & Carstensen, this volume; Baltes & Baltes, 1990). Social representations of “optimal development” or “successful aging,” ethical and religious systems, as well as philosophical teachings of wisdom often advance rules and recommendations as to how and for which purposes scarce lifetime resources should be used. Seneca’s treatise, On the Shortness of Life (De brevitate vitae) is a classical example. The fading of life time affects the construction of meaning; evaluating actions in terms of future personal consequences becomes questionable when no personal future remains. An individual’s life, as a singular, temporally closed biographical gestalt, can apparently be evaluated only with reference to values and meanings that transcend the limited horizon of one’s personal biography.

Centering on the notion of time, these introductory arguments indicate how causal and intentional aspects are intertwined in development and aging. The causal dynamics of development unfold in physical time; perceived temporal resources structure the ways that developing subjects shape and plan their personal development and aging within the constraints of causal contingencies. Development creates and shapes intentionality and intentional strivings, and developing intentionality in turn shapes the course of personal development: in that particular sense, human ontogeny is a self-referential process (Brandtstadter, 1998; Brandtstädter & Lerner, 1999).

These arguments also apply to the process of aging in its physical, social, and psychological aspects. Our behavior and development—as well as the selective, compensatory, and optimizing activities through which we try to adjust our behavior and development to normative projections (Baltes, 1997; Baltes & Baltes, 1990)—is shaped not only by the causal implications of having a particular age and a particular biographical history but also by the practical conclusions that we draw from having a given age, a given position in the life cycle, and particular physical and temporal resources. The processes of aging destroy action resources through time-contingent causal processes (such as functional losses due to changes in the processing speed and reliability of the neural network, in endocrine regulations, in functional reserves); death as a biological phenomenon occurs where these changes exceed critical margins. The adaptive challenges of age and aging, however, essentially arise from projecting these changes into temporally structured intentional spheres and relating them to strivings and desired developmental outcomes. When we ask, for example, why (or why not) some people become depressed in later life, we have to consider these projections and links.

In the following, we present theoretical arguments and research findings that bear on these issues. The theoretical approach that has guided our research focuses on the interplay of two processes—first, on intentional activities through which people shape their development in accordance with their goals and maintain desired self-attributes and, second, on mechanisms that coadjust personal goals and self-definitions to a changing field of resources and constraints (see also Brandtstädter & Greve, 1994; Brandtstädter, Wentura, & Rothermund, 1999).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bak, P. M, & Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Flexible Zielanpassung und hartnäckige Zielverfolgung als Bewältigungsressourcen: Hinweise auf ein Regulationsdilemma. Zeitschrift für Psychologic 206, 235–249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as a foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, P. (1995). Seelische Gesundheit und Verhaltenskontrolle. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L., Berger, B., & Kellner, H. (1967). The homeless mind: Modernization and unconsciousness. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, E. (1969). Philosophische Aufsätze zur objektiven Phantasie (Gesamtausgabe, Vol. 10). Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (1989). Personal self-regulation of development: Cross-sequential analyses of development-related control beliefs and emotions. Developmental Psychology, 25, 96–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Theoretical models of human development, Vol. 1, Handbook of child psychology (5th ed., pp. 807–863). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (2001). Entwicklung, Intentionalität, Handeln. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J. (in press). Agency in developmental settings of modernity: The dialectics of commitment and disengagement. American Behavioral Scientist.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Greve, W. (1994). The aging self: Stabilizing and protective processes. Developmental Review, 14, 52–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter J., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (1999). Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life span. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging, 5, 58–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2002a). Intentional self-development: Exploring the interfaces between development, intentionality, and the self. In L. J. Crockett (Ed.), Agency, motivation, and the life course (Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 48, pp. 31–75). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Rothermund, K. (2002b). The life-course dynamics of goal pursuit and goal adjustment: A two-process framework. Developmental Review, 22, 117–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., Rothermund, K., & Schmitz, U. (1997). Coping resources in later life. European Review of Applied Psychology, 47, 107–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Wentura, D. (1994). Veränderungen der Zeit- und Zukunftsperspektive im Übergang zum höheren Erwachsenenalter: entwicklungspsychologische und differentielle Aspekte. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie and Pädagogische Psychologie, 26, 2–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Wentura, D. (1995). Adjustment to shifting possibility frontiers in later life: Complementary adaptive modes. In R. A. Dixon & L. Bäckman (Eds.), Compensating for psychological deficits and declines: Managing losses and promoting gains (pp. 83–106). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Rothermund, K. (1999). Intentional self-development through adulthood and later life: Tenacious pursuit and flexible adjustment of goals. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Action and self-development: Theory and research through the life-span (pp. 373–400). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D., & Schmitz, U. (1997). Veränderungen der Zeit- und Zukunftsperspektive im Übergang zum höheren Alter: quer- und längsschnittliche Befunde. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 205, 377–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1990). Culture and human development: A new look. Human Development, 33, 344–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K. A. (1990). Peak performance and age: An examination of peak performance in sports. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 154–196). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fries, J. F. (1990). Medical perspectives upon successful aging. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 35–49). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. Selected essays. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehlen, A. (1988). Man, his nature and place in the world. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geppert, U. (1997). Persönlichkeit, Motive, soziale Kognition. In Max-Planck-Institut für psychologische Forschung (Hrsg.), Forschungsbericht 19951997 (S. 26–31). Munich: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C, Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998). Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 617–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gutmann, D. (1998). The psychoimmune system in later life. The problem of late-onset disorders. In J. Lomranz (Ed.), Handbook of aging and mental health: An integrative approach (pp. 281–295). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new psychology of trauma. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Fredrickson, B. L., Schreiber, C. A., & Redelmeier, D. A. (1993). When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science, 4, 401–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kessler, R. C, Foster, C, Webster, P. S., & House, J. S. (1992). The relationship between age and depressive symptoms in two national surveys. Psychology and Aging, 7, 119–126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational states. In F. Halisch & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, intention and volition (pp. 279–291). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1953). Zeitperspektive und Moral. In G. W. Lewin (Ed.), Die Lösung sozialer Konflikte (pp. 152–180). Bad Nauheim: Christian-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mannheim, K. (1964). Das Problem der Generationen. In K. H. Wolff (Ed.), Mannheim, Karl: Wissenssoziologie. Auswahl aus dem Werk (pp. 509–565). Neuwied: Luchterhand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marquard, O. (1984). Entlastungen. Theodizeemotive in der neuzeitlichen Philosophic In P. Wapnewski (Hrsg.), Wissenschaftskolleg. Jahrbuch 1982/93 (pp. 245–258). Berlin: Siedler.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marquard, O. (1999). Am Ende, nicht am Ziel. Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung, 273, 49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1992). Age and depression. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 187–205

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mittelstrass, J. (1992). Zeitformen des Lebens: Philosophische Unterscheidungen. In P. B. Baltes & J. Mittelstrass (Eds.), Zukunft des Alterns und gesellschaftliche Entwicklung (pp. 386–407). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. (1961). The measurement of life satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology, 16, 134–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nozick, R. (1989). The examined life. Philosophical meditations. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nuttin, J. R. (1964). The future time perspective in human motivation and learning. Acta Psychologica, 23, 60–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogilvie, D. M., & Rose, K. M. (1995). Self-with-other representations and a taxonomy of motives: Two approaches to studying persons. Journal of Personality, 63, 643–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Prelec, D., & Loewenstein, G. (1997). Beyond time discounting. Marketing Letters, 8, 97–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reker, G. T., Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. (1987). Meaning and the purpose in life and well-being: A life-span perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 42, 44–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T, & Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory: I. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 681–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K. (1998). Persistenz und Neuorientierung: Mechanismen der Aufrechterhaltung und Auflösung zielbezogener kognitiver Einstellungen. Doctoral dissertation, University of Trier, Trier, Germany.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (1997a). Entwicklung und Bewältigung: Festhalten und Preisgeben von Zielen als Formen der Bewältigung von Entwicklungszielen. In C. Tesch-Römer, C. Salewski, & G. Schwarz (Eds.), Psychologie der Bewältigung (pp. 120–133). Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (1997b). Zum Verstandnis der Assimilations- Akkommodations-Theorie. In C. Tesch-Römer, C. Salewski, & G. Schwarz (Eds.), Psychologie der Bewältigung (pp. 162–171). Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Auswirkungen von Belastungen und Verlusten auf die Lebensqualität: Alters- und lebenszeitgebundene Moderationseffekte. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie, 27, 86–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., & Brandtstädter, J. (in press). Depression in later life: Cross-sequential patterns and possible determinants. Psychology and Aging.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothermund, K., Dillmann, U., & Brandtstädter, J. (1994). Belastende Lebenssituationen im mittleren und höheren Erwachsenenalter: Zur differentiellen Wirksamkeit assimilativer und akkommodativer Bewältigung. Zeitschrift fur Gesundheitspsychologie, 2, 245–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sennett, R. (1998). The corrosion of character. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiek, J. I., & Yesavage, J. A. (1986). Geriatric depression scale (GDS): Recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clinical Gerontology, 5, 165–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., & Dittmann-Kohli, F. (1992). Lebenserfahrung und Lebenssinn. In P. B. Baltes & J. Mittelstrass (Eds.), Zukunft des Alterns und gesellschaftliche Entwicklung (pp. 408–436). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resilience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and limits of development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 2, Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 801–847). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steverink, N., Lindenberg, S., & Ormel, J. (1998). Towards understanding successful ageing: Patterned change in resources and goals. Ageing and Society, 18, 441–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 213–226.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turk-Charles, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (1999). The role of time in the setting of social goals across the life span. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging (pp. 319–339). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Valvanne, J., Juva, K., Erkinjuntti, T, & Tilvis, R. (1996). Major depression in the elderly: A population study in Helsinki. International Psychogeriatrics, 8, 437–443.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wentura, D. (1995). Verfügbarkeit entlastender Kognitionen. Zur Verarbeitung negativer Lebenssituationen. Weinheim: Psychologie Verlags Union.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyler, A. R., Masuda, M., & Holmes, T. H. (1968). Seriousness of illness rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 363–374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2003 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Brandtstädter, J., Rothermund, K. (2003). Intentionality and Time in Human Development and Aging: Compensation and Goal Adjustment in Changing Developmental Contexts. In: Staudinger, U.M., Lindenberger, U. (eds) Understanding Human Development. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-0357-6_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4020-7383-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4615-0357-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics