Skip to main content

Connectionism, Classical Cognitive Science and Experimental Psychology

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Artificial Intelligence and Society ((HCS))

Abstract

There has been an enduring tension in modern cognitive psychology between the computational models available and the experimental data obtained. Standard computational models have assumed the symbolic paradigm: that it is constitutive of cognitive processes that they are mediated by the manipulation of symbolic structures. Such schemes easily handle formal inferences, and memory for arbitrary symbolic material. However, context-sensitive defeasible inference and content-addressable memory retrieval have remained problematic. By contrast, in the empirical data on human memory and inference, the opposite profile is observed. Everyday mundane reasoning is both context dependent and defeasible, and yet is performed easily and naturally, whereas subjects are typically unable to perform the simplest formal reasoning task (Wason and Johnson-Laud 1972; Evans 1982). In memory, content-addressable access in knowledge-rich domains seems natural and unproblematic for human subjects, whereas people can retain only very small quantities of arbitrary material. Despite this tension between experiment and theory, Fodor and Pylyshyn (1988) have recently reaffirmed what they term the “classical symbolic paradigm”. That is, they argue that symbolic cognitive processes are autonomous from their implementation. Thus they question the relevance of connectionist theorizing for psychology, and suggest that connectionism should be viewed as a theory of implementation for autonomous classical architectures.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Baddeley AD (1976) The psychology of memory. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett FC (1932) Remembering: a study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobrow DG, Norman DA (1975) Some principles of memory schemata. In: Bobrow DG, Collins AM (eds) Representation and understanding: studies in cognitive science. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Bower GH (1970) Analysis of a mnemonic device. Am Scientist 58: 496–510

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford JD, Johnson M (1972) Contextual prerequisites for understanding: some investigations of comprehension and recall. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav 11: 717–726

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford JD, Johnson MK (1973) Considerations of some problems of comprehension. In: Chase WG (ed) Visual information processing. Academic Press, New York, pp 389–392

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford JD, McCarrell NS (1975) A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension: some thoughts about what it means to comprehend. In: Weimer WB, Palermo DS (eds) Cognition and symbolic processes, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 189–229

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford JD, Barclay JR, Franks JJ (1972) Sentence memory: a constructive versus interpretive approach. Cognitive Psychol 3: 193–209

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chater N, Oaksford MR (1990) Autonomy, implementation and cognitive architecture: a reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn. Cognition 34: 93–107

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW, Holyoak KJ (1985) Pragmatic reasoning schema. Cognitive Psychol 17: 391–416

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheng PW, Holyoak KJ, Nisbett RE, Oliver LM (1986) Pragmatic versus syntactic approaches in training deductive reasoning. Cognitive Psychol 18: 293–328

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark HH, Haviland SE (1977) Comprehension and the given-new contract. In: Freedle RO (ed) Discourse production and comprehension, Vol 1. Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp 1–40

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins AM, Quillian MR (1969) Retrieval time from semantic memory. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav 11: 671–684

    Google Scholar 

  • Craik FIM, Lockhart RS (1972) Levels of processing: a frame work for memory research. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav 11: 671–684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Kleer J (1986) Extending the ATMS. Artif Intell 28: 163–196

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans JStBT (1982) The psychology of deductive reasoning. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1975) The language of thought. Crowell, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1980) Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behav Brain Sci 3: 63–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1983) Modularity of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1987) Psychosemantics: the problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA, Pylyshyn ZW (1988) Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis. Cognition 28: 3–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA, Bever TG, Garrett MF (1974) The psychology of language. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Griggs RA, Cox JR (1982) The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason’s selection task. Br J Psychol 73: 407–420

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman G (1986) Change in view. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton GE (1981) Implementing semantic networks in parallel hardware. Parallel models of associative memory. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton GE (1987) Learning distributed representations of concepts. In: Proceedings of the Eighth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinton GE, McClelland JL, Rumelhart DE (1986) Distributed representations. In: Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructures of cognition. Vol 1, Foundations. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Israel DJ (1980) What’s wrong with non-monotonic logic? In: Proceedings of AAAI-80, pp 99–101

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laud PN (1986) Reasoning without logic. In: Myers T, Brown K, McGonigle B (eds) Reasoning and discourse processes. Academic Press, London, pp 13–51

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laud PN, Steedman MJ (1978) The psychology of syllogisms. Cognitive Psychol 10: 64–99

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laud PN, Wason PC (1970) A theoretical analysis of insight into a reasoning task. Cognitive Psychol 1: 134–148

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn T (1970) The structure of the scientific revolutions, 2nd ed. University of Chicago Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakatos I (1970) Falsification and the methodology of research programmes. In: Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press, pp 91–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsay PH, Norman DA (1977) Human information processing, 2nd edn. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lorayne H, Lucas J (1974) The memory book. Stein and Day, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Masterman M (1970) The nature of a paradigm. In: Lakatos I, Musgrave A (eds) Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy J (1980) Circumscription: a form of non-monotonic reasoning. Artif Intell 13: 27–39

    Article  MATH  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  • McDermott D (1986) A critique of pure reason. Technical report, Department of Computer Science, Yale University, June

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA (1956) The magical number seven plus or minus two, or, some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev 63: 81–96

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller GA, Selfridge JA (1950) Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. Am J Psychol 63: 176–185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky M (1975) Frame-system theory. In: Schank R, Nash-Webber BL (eds) Theoretical issues in natural language processing. Cambridge, MA, 10–13 June

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy GL, Medin DL (1985) The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychol Rev 92: 289–316

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1975) On data-limited and resource limited processes. Cognitive Psychol 7: 44–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1976) On the role of active memory processes in perception and cognition. In: Cofer CN (ed) The structure of human memory. Freeman, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1979) Descriptions: an intermediate stage in memory retrieval. Cognitive Psychol 11: 107–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford M (1988) Cognition and inquiry: the pragmatics of conditional reasoning. Unpublished PhD thesis, Centre for Cognitive Science, University of Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford M, Chater N (1991) Against logicist cognitive science. Mind Lang 6: 1–38

    Google Scholar 

  • Oaksford M, Stenning K (1988) Process and pragmatics in reasoning with conditionals containing negated constituents. Presented to the International Conference on Thinking, University of Aberdeen, August

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard P, Evans JStBT (1981) The effects of prior belief in reasoning: an associational interpretation. Br J Psychol 72: 73–82

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam H (1974) The “corrobation” of theories. In: Schilpp PA (ed) The philosophy of Karl Popper, Vol I. Open Court, La Salle, IL, pp 221–240

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn ZW (1973) What the mind’s eye tells the mind’s brain: a critique of mental imagery. Psychol Bull 80: 1–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn ZW (1984) Computation and cognition: toward a foundation for cognitive science. Bradford, Montgomery, VT

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiter R (1985) On reasoning by default. In: Brachman R, Levesque H (eds) Readings in knowledge representation. Kaufman, Los Altos, CA

    Google Scholar 

  • Rumelhart DE, Smolensky P, McClelland JL, Hinton GE (1986) Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In: McLelland JL, Rumelhart DE (eds) Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the micro-structures of cognition. Vol 2, Psychological and biological processes. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp 7–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard RN (1967) Recognition memory for words, sentences and pictures. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav 6: 156–163

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenning K, Levy J (1988) Knowledge-rich solutions to the binding problem: a simulation of some human computational mechanisms. Knowledge Based Syst 1: 143–152

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stenning K, Shepherd M, Levy J (1988) On the construction of representations for individuals from descriptions in text. Lang Cognitive Processes 2: 129–164

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wason PC (1966) Reasoning. In: Foss B (ed) New horizons in psychology. Penguin, Harmondsworth

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason PC, Johnson-Laird PN (1972) Psychology of reasoning: structure and content. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Wason PC, Shapiro D (1971) Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Q J Exp Psychol: Hum Exp Psychol 23: 63–71

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young NN, Gibson WB (1962) How to develop an exceptional memory. Chilton, Radnor, PA

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 Springer-Verlag London Limited

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Oaksford, M., Chater, N., Stenning, K. (1992). Connectionism, Classical Cognitive Science and Experimental Psychology. In: Clark, A., Lutz, R. (eds) Connectionism in Context. Artificial Intelligence and Society. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-1923-4_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-1923-4_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-19716-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4471-1923-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics