Skip to main content
Log in

Detection of Threats under Inattentional Blindness and Perceptual Load

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Inattentional blindness refers to the failure to detect the salient unexpected stimuli in one’s visual field when performing an attention-demanding task. The present study examined the detection of threats in a static inattentional blindness paradigm. And the detection rates of evolutionary and ontogenetic threats were compared. Participants counted the number of color words from among three (low load) or six (high load) items presented in a circular array. On the last of six trials, an unexpected threatening/neutral illustration was presented in the center of the array along with the task stimuli. Participants detection of the illustration were thus measured and analyzed. The results suggested that: (1) the threatening stimuli, both evolutionary and ontogenetic, were detected more frequently than non-threatening stimuli; (2) the unexpected illustrations were identified more frequently under low-load condition than under high-load condition; (3) even under high-load situation, the threatening illustrations were more frequently identified than neutral ones. Threats are more likely to be detected under inattentional blindness and perceptual load.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aucote, H. M., Miner, A., & Dahlhaus, P. (2012). Interpretation and misinterpretation of warning signage: Perceptions of rockfalls in a naturalistic setting. Psychology, Health and Medicine, 17(5), 522–529.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., D’Amico, S., Jacobsen, T., Székely, A., Andonova, E., Devescovi, A.,.. . Tzeng, O. (2003). Timed picture naming in seven languages. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 10(2), 344–380.

  • Beanland, V., & Chan, E. H. (2016). The relationship between sustained inattentional blindness and working memory capacity. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(3), 808–817. doi:10.3758/s13414-015-1027-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I. (2006). Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: how specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) 59(8), 1484–1504.

  • Boyer, P., & Bergstrom, B. (2011). Threat-detection in child development: An evolutionary perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(4), 1034–1041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C., El-Deredy, W., & Blanchette, I. (2010). Attentional Modulation of Visual-Evoked Potentials by Threat: Investigating the Effect of Evolutionary Relevance. Brain and Cognition, 74(3), 281–287.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., Hoffman, K. M., Payne, B. K., & Trawalter, S. (2014). The invisible man: Interpersonal goals moderate inattentional blindness to African Americans. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 143(1), 33–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvillo, D. P., & Jackson, R. E. (2014). Animacy, perceptual load, and inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(3), 670–675. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0543-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo, M., Avero, P., & Lundqvist, D. (2006). Facilitated detection of angry faces: Initial orienting and processing efficiency. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 785–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chabris, C. F., Weinberger, A., Fontaine, M., & Simons, D. J. (2011). You do not talk about fight Club if you do not notice fight Club: inattentional blindness for a simulated real-world assault. Iperception, 2(2), 150–153. doi:10.1068/i0436.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Coker-Appiah, D. S., White, S. F., Clanton, R., Yang, J., Martin, A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2013). Looming animate and inanimate threats: The response of the amygdala and periaqueductal gray. Social Neuroscience, 8(6), 621–630.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12(1), 201–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Griggs, L., & Mouchlianitis, E. (2007). The detection of fear-relevant stimuli: are guns noticed as quickly as snakes? Emotion, 7(4), 691–696. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.691.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Giesbrecht, B., Sy, J., Bundesen, C., & Kyllingsbaek, S. (2014). A new perspective on the perceptual selectivity of attention under load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 71–86. doi:10.1111/nyas.12404.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes-Hallett, A., Mayer, E. K., Marcus, H. J., Pratt, P., Mason, S., Darzi, A. W., et al. (2015). Inattention blindness in surgery. Surgical Endoscopy, 29(11), 3184–3189. doi:10.1007/s00464-014-4051-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyman Jr., I. E., Sarb, B. A., & Wise-Swanson, B. M. (2014). Failure to see money on a tree: inattentional blindness for objects that guided behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 356. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00356.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koivisto, M., & Revonsuo, A. (2009). The effects of perceptual load on semantic processing under inattention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(5), 864–868. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.5.864.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreitz, C., Furley, P., Memmert, D., & Simons, D. J. (2015). Inattentional blindness and individual differences in cognitive abilities. PloS One, 10(8), e0134675. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134675.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21(3), 451.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, N., Hirst, A., de Fockert, J. W., & Viding, E. (2004). Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 133(3), 339–354. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, Y., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2007). Word naming and psycholinguistic norms: Chinese. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 192–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2010). What's so scary about needles and knives? Examining the role of experience in threat detection. Cognition & Emotion, 24(1), 180–187. doi:10.1080/02699930802542308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: the relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion, 14(4), 701–711. doi:10.1037/a0035898.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most: a developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33(4), 285–303. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2013.07.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LoBue, V., Matthews, K., Harvey, T., & Stark, S. L. (2014). What accounts for the rapid detection of threat? Evidence for an advantage in perceptual and behavioral responding from eye movements. Emotion, 14(4), 816–823. doi:10.1037/a0035869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, M.-L. C., Mentzelb, H.-J., Miltnera, W. H. R., & Straubec, T. (2013). Amygdala activation to fearful faces under attentional load. Behavioural Brain Research, 172–175.

  • Murphy, G., & Greene, C. M. (2015). High perceptual load causes inattentional blindness and deafness in drivers. Visual Cognition, 23(7), 810–814. doi:10.1080/13506285.2015.1093245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nairne, J. S., Thompson, S. R., & Pandeirada, J. N. (2007). Adaptive memory: survival processing enhances retention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 33(2), 263–273.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norberg, J., Peira, N., & Wiens, S. (2010). Never mind the spider: Late positive potentials to phobic threat at fixation are unaffected by perceptual load. Psychophysiology, 47(6), 1151–1158.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A., Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: a threat advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 80(3), 381–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pammer, K., Bairnsfather, J., Burns, J., & Hellsing, A. (2015). Not all hazards are created equal: the significance of hazards in inattentional blindness for static driving scenes. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 782–788. doi:10.1002/acp.3153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, A., Cartwright-Finch, U., & Lavie, N. (2014). I can see clearly now: the effects of age and perceptual load on inattentional blindness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 229. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00229.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, M. G., Eastwood, J. D., Partanen, M., Frischen, A., & Smilek., D. (2009). Monitoring eye movements while searching for affective faces. Visual Cognition, 17(3), 318–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, A., Hellgren, M. G., & French, C. C. (2014). Inattentional blindness, absorption, working memory capacity, and paranormal belief. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 60–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). <a standardized set of 260 pictures norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity >. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning & Memory, 6(2), 174–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soares, S. C., & Esteves, F. (2013). A glimpse of fear: fast detection of threatening targets in visual search with brief stimulus durations. Psych J, 2(1), 11–16. doi:10.1002/pchj.18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wheaton, M. G., Fitzgerald, D. A., Phan, K. L., & Klumpp, H. (2014). Perceptual load modulates anterior cingulate cortex response to threat distractors in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychology, 101, 13–17.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In I. V. C. (Ed.) (Ed.), Fleeting memories (pp. 71–94). Cambridge: MIT Press.

  • Yang, J., Bellgowan, P. S. F., & Martin, A. (2012). Threat, domain-specificity and the human amygdala. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2566–2572.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Yuan, L., Ma, Y.-f., Lei, Z.-y., & Xu, P. (2014). Driver’s comprehension and improvement of warning signs. Advances in Mechanical Engineering. 1-9. doi:10.1155/2014/582606.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the two anonymous reviewers for their comments on the early version of this work.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hua Gao.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

There is no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gao, H., Jia, Z. Detection of Threats under Inattentional Blindness and Perceptual Load. Curr Psychol 36, 733–739 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9460-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9460-0

Keywords

Navigation