Skip to main content
Log in

Charitable Intent: A Moral or Social Construct? A Revised Theory of Planned Behavior Model

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Given an increasing global need to elicit and stimulate charitable giving and in light of the limited social-psychological research on this subject, this study contributes to a better empirical understanding of the factors that underlie charitable giving (intentions). In contrast to previous research, it was hypothesized that moral norms rather than social norms are likely to play a significant role in the formation of charitable intentions. An extended Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) model was constructed in order to test the influence of six social-psychological variables on an individual’s intention to donate to charity. Respondents (N = 143) completed an online questionnaire about charitable behaviour that assessed the constructs of the revised model. The present study found support for the stated hypothesis: while social norms (both descriptive and prescriptive) did not explain any of the variance in intention, moral norms accounted for a significant amount of the overall variance and were in fact identified as the strongest (relative) predictor of charitable giving intentions. In addition to moral norms, ‘attitude’, ‘perceived behavioral control’ and ‘past behaviour’ were also identified as significant predictors. The findings in this study support a revised TPB model that accounts for nearly 70% of the explained variance in charitable intent. Implications for both theory and practice are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Of course, in some situations when it remains relatively unclear what the appropriate norm is, it is likely that people will look at others for clues (e.g., Festinger 1954).

  2. Even if some correlations (.61) are to be considered relatively high, an examination of the collinearity statistics revealed that each predictor fell within acceptable boundaries of tolerance (>.3) and the VIF coefficient (<5), ruling out any substantive multi-collinearity problems (O’Brien 2007).

References

  • Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen. (2002). Residual effects of past on later behavior. Habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 107–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ajzen, I. (2006). Theory of planned behavior. Constructing A TPB Questionnaire. Retrieved on December 21st, 2010 from: http://www.people.umass.edu/aizen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf.

  • Albarracín, D., Johnson, B. T., & Zanna, M. P. (2005). The handbook of attitudes. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armon, C., & Dawson, T. L. (1997). Developmental trajectories in moral reasoning across the life span. Journal of Moral Education, 26(4), 433–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beck, L., & Ajzen, I. (1991). Predicting dishonest actions using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 25, 285–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C. (2006). The grammar of society. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borgloh, S., Dannenberg, A., and Aretz, B. (2010). Small is beautiful: Experimental evidence of donor’s preferences for charities. ZEW Discussion Paper, No. 10–052.

  • Brooks, A. C. (2003). Religious faith and charitable giving. Policy Review, 121. Retrieved on November 19th, 2011 from: http://www.hoover.org/publications/policy-review/article/6577.

  • Brooks, A. C. (2007). Does giving make us prosperous? Journal of Economics and Finance, 31(3), 403–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgoyne, C. B., Young, B., & Walker, C. M. (2005). Deciding to give to charity: A focus group study in the context of the household economy. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 15, 383–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Canadian Centre for Philanthropy (2004). Understanding Canadian donors: Using the national survey of giving, volunteering and participating to build your fundraising program. Retrieved on December 19th, 2010, from: www.givingandvolunteering.ca/pdf/reports/Understanding_Donors.pdf.

  • Charities Aid Foundation (2010). The world giving index. Retrieved on August 21nd, 2011, from: https://www.cafonline.org/pdf/WorldGivingIndex28092010Print.pdf.

  • Cialdini, R. B. (2005). Basic social influence is underestimated. Psychological Inquiry, 16(4), 158–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B. (2007). Descriptive social norms as underappreciated sources of social control. Psychometrika, 72(2), 263–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of normative conduct. Advances in Experimental Psychology, 24, 201–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conner, M., & Armitage, C. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Croson, R., & Shang, J. (2008). The impact of downward social information on contribution decisions. Experimental Economics, 11, 221–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational influences upon individual judgement. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 629–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drollinger, T. L. (1998). A multidisciplinary model of monetary donations to charitable organisations. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 59, 0458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, B. (2004). A theory of impact philanthropy. Journal of Public Economics, 88, 2159–2180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, A. M. (2009). Insufficient discriminant validity: A comment on Bove, Pervan, Beatty and Shiu. Journal of Business Research, 63(3), 324–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrell, A. M. and Rudd, J. M. (2009). Factor analysis and discriminant validity: A brief review of some practical issues. Working Paper: Australia-New Zeeland Marketing Academy Conference (ANZMAC). Melbourne, Australia.

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B. S., & Meier, S. (2004). Social comparisons and pro-social behavior: Testing “conditional cooperation” in a field experiment. American Economic Review, 94, 1717–1722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gorsuch, R. L., & Ortberg, J. (1983). Moral obligation and attitudes: Their relation to behavioral intentions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1025–1028.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • John, O. P., & Benet-Martinez, V. (2000). Measurement: Reliability, construct validation, and scale construction. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology (pp. 339–369). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, G. F., Hübner, G., & Bogner, X. F. (2005). Contrasting the theory of planned behavior with the value-belief-norm model in explaining conservation behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35(10), 2150–2170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, D., and Janicki, M. (2002). Biological foundations of moral norms. In M. Schaller and C. Crandall (Eds.), Psychological Foundations of Culture.

  • Kurland, N. B. (1995). Ethical intentions and the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 297–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, L., Piliavin, J. A., & Call, V. R. (1999). Giving time, money, and blood: Similarities and differences. Social Psychology Quarterly, 62, 276–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). The role of moral norm in the attitude–behavior relation. In D. J. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitude, behavior, and social context: The role of norms and group membership (pp. 11–30). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R., & Randal, J. (2008). How is donation behavior affected by the donations of others? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization., 67, 228–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, K. (2005). Communications theories; perspectives, processes, and contexts. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, J., Krueger, F., Zhn, R., Pardini, M., Oliveira-Souze, R., & Grafman, J. (2006). Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, J. M., Schultz, W., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). Normative social influence is underdetected. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 913.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, R. M. (2007). A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality and Quantity, 41(5), 673–690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: The multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 124(1), 54–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Stradling, S. G. (1995). Extending the theory of planned behavior: The role of personal norm. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 127–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pharoah, C., & Tanner, S. (1997). Trends in charitable giving. Fiscal Studies, 18, 427–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radley, A., & Kennedy, M. (1995). Charitable giving by individuals: A study of attitudes and practice. Human Relations, 48, 685–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reno, R. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Kallgren, C. A. (1993). The transitional influence of social norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 104–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social, 22, 218–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosen, H. S., and Sims, S. T. (2010). Altruistic behavior and habit formation. Princeton’s Center for Economic Policy Studies. Working Paper No. 210.

  • Sargeant, A., & Lee, S. (2004). Trust and relationship commitment in the United Kingdom voluntary sector: Determinants of donor behavior. Psychology and Marketing, 21, 613–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schervish, P. G., & Havens, J. (1997). Social participation and charitable giving: A multivariate analysis. Voluntas, 8, 235–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. P., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2007). The constructive, destructive and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science, 18(5), 429–434.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influences on altruism. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, 10 (pp. 222–280). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), European review of social psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 1–30). Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. R., & McSweeney, A. (2007). Charitable giving: The effectiviness of a revised theory of planned behavior model in predicting donating intentions and behavior. Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology, 17, 363–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., Abel, T., Guagnano, N., & Kalof, L. (1999). A value-belief-norm theory of support for social movements: The case of environmentalism. Human Ecology Review, 6(2), 81–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, S. (1998). Predicting and explaining intentions and behavior: How well are we doing? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1317–1338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1996). Group norms and the attitude-behavior relationship: A role for group identification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 776–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, D. (2004). Problems with change in R2 as applied to theory of reasoned action research. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 515–530.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance of subjective norms for a minority of people: Between subjects and within-subjects analyses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 820–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B. (2006). Beyond frequency: Habit as mental construct. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, 639–656.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verplanken, B., & Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned behavior: Is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of automaticity? European Review of Social Psychology, 10, 101–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Warburton, J., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Volunteer decision making by older people: A test of a revised theory of planned behavior. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 22, 245–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, M. K., Smith, R. J., Terry, J. D., Greenslade, H. J., & McKimmie, M. B. (2009). Social influence in the theory of planned behavior: The role of descriptive, injunctive and in-group norms. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 135–138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sander van der Linden.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

van der Linden, S. Charitable Intent: A Moral or Social Construct? A Revised Theory of Planned Behavior Model. Curr Psychol 30, 355–374 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9122-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-011-9122-1

Keywords

Navigation