INTRODUCTION

Prior research has demonstrated that black patients, on average, prefer the care they receive from physicians of their own race.1,2,3,4 This finding is often presumed to reflect better interpersonal care, including more effective communication and culturally appropriate behavior, from physicians whose shared culture and experience promote greater understanding of patients’ preferences and communication styles.2,5 This theory has provided one rationale for “cultural competence” training, intended to “bridge the gap” between physicians and patients from different backgrounds.6

Another possible explanation for the effects of patient-physician race concordance is that black patients have better experiences with black physicians, independent of physician behavior, because they trust other black people more and view them as less likely to perpetrate discrimination. Numerous studies have shown that people have higher trust and comfort with “ingroup” than with “outgroup” members.7,8

If physician race affects patient experience and trust, it may also affect patient decision-making in a way that contributes to disparities in health care.9 Prior studies have shown that physicians perceive patients differently and make different clinical decisions based on non-clinical factors such as race, gender, and social class.10,11,12,13 It is plausible that these perceptions also work in the opposite direction, such that physician characteristics affect patients’ decision-making. Understanding the degree to which interpersonal barriers between patients and physicians result from fixed characteristics, such as race, versus modifiable characteristics, such as communication skills and cultural sensitivity, is an important step towards elucidating the causes of disparities in health care and the best avenues to reduce them.

We conducted a randomized experimental study, using standardized video vignettes, to determine whether physician race affects patients’ perceptions and decision-making, independent of physician behavior. We additionally examined whether any effects of physician race were modified by other characteristics, particularly the physician’s communication style.

METHODS

Vignette Development

Our video vignette portrayed a visit between a physician and a patient with progressive angina who recently underwent coronary angiography revealing 3-vessel coronary artery disease (CAD).14 The physician reviews the angiogram results and recommends coronary artery bypass graft (CABG) surgery, citing evidence of improved quality of life and long-term survival. The patient expresses fear about the surgery. The physician provides reassurance, citing statistics about perioperative risks. A discussion ensues about treatment alternatives, including percutaneous intervention and medical management. The physician closes by reiterating the recommendation of CABG.

We chose this scenario for several reasons. First, CABG for chronic angina and 3-vessel CAD is supported by clinical trials and guidelines, making the physician’s recommendation evidence-based.15 Second, racial disparities in receipt of CABG have been well documented and have persisted for decades.16,17,18 Third, there are substantial risks to either undergoing or foregoing CABG in the setting of 3-vessel CAD. This high-stakes context increases the relevance of patients’ trust and confidence in the physician’s recommendation. Finally, evidence shows that patients rely heavily on physician recommendations in weighing options for treating CAD.19

Because we were also interested in exploring the impact of communication style, we created two versions of the vignette, with the physician using a low vs. high patient-centered communication (PCC) style. We first chose two core principles of PCC: a biopsychosocial orientation and a partnered approach in doctor-patient interactions.20 We then chose verbal and non-verbal communication behaviors commonly mentioned as ways of promoting these ideals (Table 1).21,22 We varied these behaviors but kept the vignettes the same in other respects, including clinical information, numeric estimates of risk and benefit, and strength of recommendation. In a previous study, our high PCC vignettes were associated with higher ratings of the video physician on multiple dimensions including communication, validating that the 2 versions captured intended differences in communication style.14

Table 1 Characteristics of Low and High Patient-Centeredness

We filmed 32 videos: 8 physician actors—each representing a unique combination of black vs. white, male vs. female, and young vs. middle-aged (Fig.1)—performing 2 vignette versions (low and high PCC), each with 2 patient voices (male and female). To facilitate study participants assuming the role of the patient, vignettes were filmed from a point-of-view angle: the patient was heard but not seen, and the physician faced the camera when speaking. Each video introduces the patient as a 55-year-old woman or man with hypertension and progressive chest pain, who had a positive exercise treadmill test leading to an angiogram. The introduction explains what an angiogram is and that this is the patient’s 2nd visit to this physician, with the purpose of discussing angiogram results.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Actors portraying physicians.

Setting, Participants, and Study Procedures

We recruited study participants at a hospital-based primary care clinic in the Pacific Northwest. We approached consecutive patients 40 or older whose records indicated a diagnosis of CAD or CAD risk factors (smoking, hypertension, diabetes, or hyperlipidemia), to increase the relevance of a vignette about CAD treatment options. Participants were told the study purpose was to examine patients’ decision-making about heart disease, and that when watching the vignette, they should place themselves in the role of the video patient. Acutely ill patients and those with cognitive or severe visual impairment were ineligible. The study was approved by the Legacy Health System Institutional Review Board.

After giving informed consent, participants completed a brief questionnaire evaluating demographic characteristics, cardiac history, familiarity with and knowledge about different CAD treatment options, experience of racial discrimination when receiving health care,23 and trust in health care.24

Randomization

Each of our 32 videos was given an alphanumeric code. Once enrolled, research assistants assigned participants to a vignette based on computer-generated randomized lists generated by the principal investigator (SS). Male and female participants were assigned to vignettes with a male or female patient voice, respectively. Research assistants were unaware of the alphanumeric codes linked to each video. The principal investigator was not involved in enrolling participants.

Outcome Measures

After viewing the assigned video, participants completed a questionnaire assessing their impressions of the video physician and the recommendation of CABG (Appendix). Participants rated the physician’s communication (5 items, Cronbach’s alpha 0.95),25 interpersonal style (5 items, alpha 0.92),26 and competence (4 items, alpha 0.84). They also reported on their trust in the video physician (10 items, alpha 0.94),27 how much they liked and felt comfortable with the physician (3 items, alpha 0.94), and their perceptions of the physician overall (1 item). Participants reported their perception of the necessity of CABG for the patient in the video (0–5 scale from absolutely unnecessary to absolutely necessary). They also rated their likelihood of undergoing CABG and of obtaining a second opinion, if they were the patient in the video, and their sense of whether the benefits of CABG outweighed the risks (all using 0–3 scale from definitely not to definitely).

Data Analysis

We used t tests and chi-square tests to compare participant characteristics by race and condition assignment (video physician race). We stratified all analyses of the effects of physician race, by participant race. To account for any within-video-physician correlations (i.e., individual actor effects), we tested statistical significance using GEE-based linear regression analyses with video physician actor as the cluster variable.

We conducted interaction analyses to test whether any observed effects of physician race were modified by communication style (high vs. low PCC). We similarly tested for interactions between physician race and physician gender, patient gender, patient-physician gender concordance, patient age, experience of discrimination, and trust in health care, to see if the impact of physician race varied by these characteristics. Where significant interactions were observed, we repeated our main analyses, stratified by levels of the modifying variable. Analyses were conducted using Stata/SE 14 (College Station, TX, USA).

RESULTS

Participant Characteristics

We approached 373 patients; 27 were ineligible and 98 declined (Fig. 2). Participants were younger than those who declined (57.8 vs. 62.3) and more likely to be black (45% vs. 41%) and female (59% vs. 52%). Among the 248 participants completing the study (72% participation rate), 10 self-identified as other than black or white. Among the remaining 238, the 107 black participants were less likely than the 131 white participants to have completed high school (78% vs. 91%, p = 0.007), to report having CAD (16% vs. 27%, p = 0.03), to have undergone CABG (0% vs. 7%, p = 0.006), to have familiarity with CABG (39% vs. 56%, p = 0.008), and to feel knowledgeable about CABG (33% vs. 50%, p = 0.006). Prior to viewing a video vignette, black participants were more likely than whites to perceive the mortality risk of CABG as above 10% (48% vs. 32%, p = 0.02). They also more frequently reported having experienced racial discrimination in healthcare settings (50% vs. 12%, p < 0.001) and had lower levels of trust in the healthcare system (2.93 vs. 3.19 on 1–5 scale, p = 0.001). Participant characteristics were balanced across randomization groups (Table 2).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Study recruitment.

Table 2 Baseline Participant Characteristics, by Video Physician Race

Ratings of Video Physicians

Black participants gave higher ratings on all physician attributes—communication, interpersonal style, competence, trustworthiness, and likability—and a superior overall rating, when the video physician was black rather than white (Table 3). For white participants, there were no significant differences by physician race.

Table 3 Ratings of Video Physician, by Participant and Video Physician Race

Decision-Making Outcomes

Black participants viewing a vignette with a black physician rated the necessity of CABG significantly higher and indicated they would be more likely to undergo CABG if they were the patient in the video, compared with black participants viewing a white physician (Table 4). Black participants who viewed a race-concordant physician also expressed a somewhat lower desire for a second opinion, though this was not statistically significant. Race concordance was not associated with CABG risk/benefit perceptions. For white participants, there were no differences in decision-making outcomes by physician race.

Table 4 Decision-making Outcomes, by Participant and Video Physician Race

Interactions of Physician Race with Other Variables

The effect of physician race on ratings and decision-making outcomes among black patients was generally less pronounced with higher PCC (Table 5). For instance, the difference in overall ratings of the black vs. white physicians was significantly greater for low vs. high PCC vignettes (1.56 vs. 0.57, p = 0.03 for physician race by PCC interaction). Similar patterns were seen with the outcomes of interpersonal style, trustworthiness, likability, and perceived necessity of CABG. Even with the high PCC vignettes, however, the effect of race on ratings of the physician and expressed likelihood of undergoing CABG remained significant.

Table 5 Ratings and Decision-Making Among Black Study Participants, by Level of Patient-Centered Communication Style and Video Physician Race

We found no interactions between video physician race and patient gender, physician gender, or patient-physician gender concordance. We did find interactions between video physician race and patient age, experience of discrimination, and trust in the healthcare system, for specific outcomes. Patient age modified the effect of race concordance on ratings of physician competence; black participants 60 and over perceived the black physicians as more competent (3.56 vs. 2.58, p < 0.001), but those under 60 did not (3.20 vs. 2.91, p = 0.17). Among black participants reporting prior discrimination, race concordance was associated with a more favorable risk/benefit tradeoff perception (2.61 vs. 2.10, p = 0.003); a trend toward the reverse finding (better outcome with race discordance) was seen among those not reporting discrimination (2.37 vs. 2.65, p = 0.051). Among black participants with less trust in health care (score < 3.5), race concordance was associated with higher perceived need for CABG (4.03 vs. 3.59, p = 0.015); this association was not observed among those with a higher trust score (4.13 vs. 4.33, p = 0.09). Finally, race concordance was associated with a more favorable perceived risk/benefit tradeoff (2.60 vs. 1.69, p = 0.001) among black participants with a trust score less than 2.5, but not among those with higher trust (2.44 vs. 2.55, p = 0.40).

DISCUSSION

In this randomized experiment, we found that black patients viewed the doctor in a scripted video vignette more positively, and were more receptive to the same recommendation, communicated in the same way, with a black rather than white physician. The effect of physician race was reduced, but not eliminated, when the physician used more PCC behaviors. For white patients, physician race did not affect patient perceptions or decision-making.

Numerous studies have demonstrated that patient-physician race concordance is associated with higher ratings of physicians and better perceived communication, particularly among black patients.2,4 It is commonly argued that these findings reflect clinical interactions with greater cultural concordance, less racial bias, or more similar communication styles between physicians and patients from the same background.2,5,6 Our findings suggest that at least part of the effect of race concordance may be unrelated to physician behavior, driven instead by experiences and perceptions shaped by influences outside the clinical encounter.

Prior studies provide supportive evidence for this hypothesis. In a study using virtual reality technology, Persky et al. found that African-Americans had more accurate perceptions of lung cancer risk after receiving information from an African-American, compared with a white, physician avatar.28 Whittle et al. found that black patients more often expressed willingness to undergo a cardiac procedure when interviewed by a black vs. white research assistant using the same survey script.29

These findings should not be surprising. Black Americans have endured systematic oppression that provides ample reason to trust members of their own community over others. The racism that gave rise to segregation, economic deprivation, mass incarceration, redlining, police brutality, and the Tuskegee syphilis study still pervades our society and its institutions, including our healthcare system.30,31,32 In this context, it is understandable that black patients might be more comfortable with black physicians, and more likely to trust their recommendations. Notably, the effect of race concordance on perceptions of the physician’s recommendation in our study was more pronounced among participants reporting prior discrimination and lower trust in health care. It should also be unsurprising that physician race did not influence white patients’ perceptions and decision-making, as they have not been on the receiving end of the racial oppression that likely underlies the impact of physician race for black patients.

Studies examining whether the effects of race concordance go beyond patient experience and perceptions have generally found that concordance does not influence other aspects of healthcare quality, including receipt of basic services.33,34 This may reflect the fact that meeting those quality metrics is driven primarily by physician decision-making, ordering, and referrals. Our findings, and those of other studies, suggest that there may be downstream effects of race concordance that are more dependent on patient decision-making, such as acceptance of high-stakes recommendations, adherence to treatment plans, and engagement in care.35,36

We strongly caution against the interpretation of our results as suggesting that minority patients should be preferentially assigned to physicians of their own race. The impact of physician race on black patients’ trust and comfort, while present on average, is not universal, and presumptions should never be made about individual patients’ preferences. We do believe, however, that in relationships as personal as that between patient and physician, where private and sensitive information is shared and high-stakes decisions are made, patients should have latitude to choose a physician with whom they feel comfortable. It has become generally accepted that women be allowed to choose a gender-concordant physician if they prefer. To the extent that race creates similar issues around patient comfort and safety, minority patients should be able to choose a race-concordant physician if they prefer.

Prior studies have shown that minority patients do disproportionately seek out physicians from their own background.3,37 Many patients, however, are unable to see a race-concordant physician, even if preferred, because of the undersupply of minority physicians. Black Americans currently make up 13% of the U.S. population38 but only 6% of physicians.39 Latinos likewise comprise 18% of the population but 5% of physicians.38,39 Given widespread racial disparities in health care, and the findings from our study suggesting that race discordance may affect trust and decision-making in ways that contribute to these disparities, remedying the underrepresentation of minority physicians should be a national priority.

Diversifying the physician workforce will take time. In the meantime, our findings provide evidence that the “social distance” between some patients and physicians in race-discordant relationships can be at least partially bridged through effective communication.40,41 While PCC behaviors are likely to enhance all relationships, they may be particularly important in the setting of race discordance or other differences that create interpersonal distance between patients and physicians. Many physicians do not believe they contribute to racial disparities because they treat all patients equally.42 But equal treatment is likely to maintain, rather than ameliorate, existing disparities. Reducing disparities may require that physicians invest more heavily in rapport and relationship building in situations, like race discordance, where there may be greater social distance to bridge than in the average clinical interaction.

Limitations

We recruited patients from a single setting with relatively few black physicians. Lack of significant prior exposure to black physicians might have influenced our results for both black and white participants. Extending this study to different regions of the country would help determine if our results are broadly generalizable. We measured patients’ reactions to a simulated scenario about a specific procedure. We used CABG as a model for reasons described above. While we believe our findings are likely to be generalizable to other high-stakes clinical decisions, this could be tested in future studies. Finally, we evaluated hypothetical, rather than real, decision-making. Prior studies, however, have found the perceptions of analog patients to be reliable and valid predictors of real patients’ perceptions.43,44 Moreover, simulated encounters confer the benefit of isolating the effects of specific variables, and in this study, we were able to study the impact of race, independent of other factors. We were also able to avoid the problem of skewed ratings when patients evaluate their own physicians.44

CONCLUSION

We found that black patients viewed black physicians more positively than white physicians, even when physicians gave the same information in the same manner. Race concordance also affected patients’ decision-making in the context of an evidence-based, high-stakes recommendation. These differences were reduced, but not eliminated, when physicians used PCC behaviors. These findings suggest that physician race may affect outcomes of patient-physician interactions in ways that are unrelated to physician behavior, and that communication or cultural competence training is likely to be only a partial solution to disparities stemming from racial barriers in patient-physician relationships. Our findings add further evidence that increasing the racial diversity of the physician workforce may help reduce disparities in health care.