Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Are Associations Between “Sexist” Video Games and Decreased Empathy Toward Women Robust? A Reanalysis of Gabbiadini et al. 2016

  • Empirical Research
  • Published:
Journal of Youth and Adolescence Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gabbiadini, A., Riva, P., Andrighetto, L., Volpato, C., & Bushman, B, (PloS ONE, 2016) provided evidence for a connection between “sexist” video games and decreased empathy toward girls using an experimental paradigm. These claims are based on a moderated mediation model. They reported a three-way interaction between game condition, gender, and avatar identification when predicting masculine ideology in their original study. Masculine ideology was associated, in turn, with decreased empathy. However, there were no main experimental effects for video game condition on empathy. The current analysis considers the strength of the evidence for claims made in the original study on a sample of 153 adolescents (M age = 16.812, SD = 1.241; 44.2% male). We confirmed that there was little evidence for an overall effect of game condition on empathy toward girls or women. We tested the robustness of the original reported moderated mediation models against other, theoretically derived alternatives, and found that effects differed based on how variables were measured (using alternatives in their public data file) and the statistical model used. The experimental groups differed significantly and substantially in terms of age suggesting that there might have been issues with the procedures used to randomly assign participants to conditions. These results highlight the need for preregistration of experimental protocols in video game research and raise some concerns about how moderated mediation models are used to support causal inferences. These results call into question whether use of “sexist” video games is a causal factor in the development of reduced empathy toward girls and women among adolescents.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Both of these games were curiously dated at the time of the study, San Andreas having been released in 2004, Vice City in 2002.

  2. This was consistent across conditions: Neutral games r = −.321 (n = 51); Violent games r = −.299 (n = 55); Sexist games r = −.429 (n = 48).

  3. During personal communication Dr. Gabbiadini explained that many items for the MRNI were not used because they used language better fit for adults than adolescents or contained language the schools considered explicit. Dr. Gabbiadini also suggested that they may have eliminated the other 3 items that were in the dataset for the same reason. We accepted this explanation as reasonable for using a reduced set of items in the original survey. However, we were not convinced that this logic applied to any decision to drop 3-items from the scale after the data had been collected.

  4. An examination of crosstabs revealed that all 15-year olds (n = 22) were in the GTA condition. Few participants from ages 17–20 (n = 4) were in the GTA condition. This grouping of younger participants into the GTA condition appears to be difficult to explain through random assignment of individuals to condition. This result is presented in Table 1. It should have also been noted in the original report given the strong negative correlation between condition in age in their Table 2 (i.e., r = −.521, n = 155).

  5. We noticed that one participant (ID# 108) was excluded from these analyses, likely because empathy data was missing for this participant. However, as the empathy data were not necessary for this particular analyses, we also conducted an analysis with #108 included. When this participant was included in the analysis the, results were not statistically significant [F (2, 152) = 2.573, p = .080] Thus, the outcome of this analysis in terms of statistical significance hinges on the presence or absence of a single participant.

  6. The meta-analysis became the focus of lively debate between scholars who supported (Markey 2015) and were critical (e.g., Rothstein and Bushman 2015; Valkenburg 2015) of its conclusions. However, the meta-analysis was independently replicated by Furuya-Kanamori and Doi (2016) who were, in turn, critical of the critiques.

References

  • Adachi, P. C., Hodson, G., Willoughby, T., Blank, C., & Ha, A. (2016). From outgroups to allied forces: Effect of intergroup cooperation in violent and nonviolent video games on boosting favorable outgroup attitudes. Journal Of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(3), 259–265. doi:10.1037/xge0000145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adachi, P. C., & Willoughby, T. (2011). The effect of violent video games on aggression: Is it more than just the violence? Aggression And Violent Behavior, 16(1), 55–62. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2010.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adachi, P. C., & Willoughby, T. (2013). Do video games promote positive youth development? Journal Of Adolescent Research, 28(2), 155–165. doi:10.1177/0743558412464522.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C. A., & Murphy, C. R. (2003). Violent video games and aggressive behavior in young women. Aggressive Behavior, 29(5), 423–429. doi:10.1002/ab.10042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barone, M. J., Shimp, T. A., & Sprott, D. E. (1997). Mere ownership revisited: A robust effect? Journal Of Consumer Psychology, 6(3), 257–284. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp0603_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boot, W., Blakely, D., & Simons, D. (2011). Do action video games improve perception and cognition? Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 226 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00226.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bowman, N. D. (2016). The rise (and refinement) of moral panic. In R. Kowert, & T. Quandt (Eds.), The video game debate: Unraveling the physical, social, and psychological effects of digital games (pp. 22–38). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breuer, J., Kowert, R., Festl, R., & Quandt, T. (2015). Sexist games=Sexist gamers? A longitudinal study on the relationship between video game use and sexist attitudes. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 18(4), 197–202. doi:10.1089/cyber.2014.0492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, S., Engelstatter, B., & Ward, M. R. (2016). Violent video games and violent crime. Southern Economic Journal, 82(4), 1247–1265.

  • Elson, M., Mohseni, M. R., Breuer, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2014). Press CRTT to measure aggressive behavior: The unstandardized use of the competitive reaction time task in aggression research. Psychological Assessment, 26, 419–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Espelage, D. L., Mebane, S. E., & Adams, M. R. (2004). Empathy, caring, and bullying: Toward an understanding of complex associations. In D. L. Espelage, S. M. Swearer (Eds.). Bullying in American schools: A social-ecological perspective in prevention and intervention (pp. 37–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. J. (2015a). Do angry birds make for angry children? A meta-analysis of video game Influences on children’s and adolescents’ aggression, mental health, prosocial behavior and academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10, 646–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. J. (2015b). “Everybody knows psychology is not a real science”: Public perceptions of psychology and how we can improve our relationship with policymakers, the scientific community, and the general public. American Psychologist, 70, 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. J., Rueda, S. M., Cruz, A. M., Ferguson, D. E., Fritz, S., & Smith, S. M. (2008). Violent video games and aggression: Causal relationship or byproduct of family violence and intrinsic violence motivation? Criminal Justice And Behavior, 35(3), 311–332. doi:10.1177/0093854807311719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A. M., & Smetana, J. (1984). Abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated preschoolers’ ability to discriminate emotions in others: The effects of IQ. Child Abuse & Neglect, 8(4), 459–465. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(84)90027-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furuya-Kanamori, L., & Doi, S. R. (2016). Angry birds, angry children, and angry meta-analysts: A reanalysis. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 11(3), 408–414. doi:10.1177/1745691616635599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gabbiadini, A., Riva, P., Andrighetto, L., Volpato,C., & Bushman, B. (2016). Acting like a tough guy: Violent-sexist video Games, identification With game characters, masculine beliefs, & empathy for female violence victims. PloS One. http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0152121.

  • Gelman, A., & Loken, E. (2013). The statistical crisis in science. American Scientist, 102, 460–465.

  • Grizzard, M., Tamborini, R., Lewis, R. J., Wang, L., & Prabhu, S. (2014). Being bad in a video game can make us morally sensitive. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, And Social Networking, 17(8), 499–504. doi:10.1089/cyber.2013.0658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2004). Empathy and offending: A systematic review and meta- analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9(5), 441–476. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2003.03.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Journal of Media Psychology (2016). Technology and human behavior: A preregistered special issue of the Journal of Media Psychology. http://www.icahdq.org/pubs/calls/JournalOfMedia.asp.

  • Furuya-Kanamori, F., & Doi, S. (2016). Angry birds, angry children and angry meta-analysts. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(3), 408–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2015). A critical account of DSM-5 criteria for internet gaming disorder. Addiction Research & Theory, 23(2), 93–98. doi:10.3109/16066359.2014.935350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J. L., & Rhee, S. H. (2008). The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: Genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion, 8(6), 737–752. doi:10.1037/a0014179.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Konijn, E. A., Nije Bijvank, M., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). I wish I were a warrior: The role of wishful identification in the effects of violent video games on aggression in adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 1038–1044. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Levant, R. F., Rankin, T. J., Williams, C. M., Hasan, N. T., & Smalley, K. B. (2010). Evaluation of the factor structure and construct validity of scores on the Male role norms inventory—Revised (MRNI-R). Psychology Of Men & Masculinity, 11(1), 25–37. doi:10.1037/a0017637.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Licata, M., Williams, A., & Paulus, M. (2016). The development of empathy in early childhood. In D. F. Watt, J. Panksepp, D. F. Watt, & J. Panksepp (Eds.), Psychology and neurobiology of empathy (pp. 111–148). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Biomedical Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markey, P. M. (2015). Finding the middle ground in violent video game research: Lessons from Ferguson (2015). Perspectives On Psychological Science, 10(5), 667–670. doi:10.1177/1745691615592236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Markey, P. M., Males, M. A., French, J. E., & Markey, C. N. (2015a). Lessons from Markey et al. (2015) and Bushman et al. (2015): Sensationalism and integrity in media research. Human Communication Research, 41(2), 184–203. doi:10.1111/hcre.12057.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markey, P. M., Markey, C. N., & French, J. E. (2015b). Violent video games and real-world violence: Rhetoric versus data. Psychology Of Popular Media Culture, 4(4), 277–295. doi:10.1037/ppm0000030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Spies, J. R., & Motyl, M. (2012). Scientific utopia: II. Restructuring incentives and practices to promote truth over publishability. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 7(6), 615–631. doi:10.1177/1745691612459058.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Przybylski, A. K., Deci, E. L., Rigby, C. S., & Ryan, R. M. (2014). Competence-impeding electronic games and players’ aggressive feelings, thoughts, and behaviors. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 106(3), 441–457. doi:10.1037/a0034820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, H. R., & Bushman, B. J. (2015). Methodological and reporting errors in meta-analytic reviews make other meta-analysts angry: A commentary on Ferguson (2015). Perspectives On Psychological Science, 10(5), 677–679. doi:10.1177/1745691615592235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Savacool, J. (2014). Think you’re immune to video-game violence? Think again. USA Today. https://www.usatoday.com/story/tech/gaming/2014/03/30/video-games-violence/6437253/.

  • Sijtsma, K. (2016). Playing with data—or how to discourage questionable research practices and stimulate researchers to do things right. Psychometrika, 81(1), 1–15. doi:10.1007/s11336-015-9446-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Singal, J. (2016). Here’s an overhyped study about video-game violence and misogyny. New York Magazine. http://nymag.com/scienceofus/2016/04/here-s-an-overhyped-study-about-video-game-violence-and-misogyny.html.

  • Stermer, S. P., & Burkley, M. (2015). SeX-Box: Exposure to sexist video games predicts benevolent sexism. Psychology Of Popular Media Culture, 4(1), 47–55. doi:10.1037/a0028397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Surette, R. (2013). Cause or catalyst: The interaction of real world and media crime models. American Journal Of Criminal Justice, 38(3), 392–409. doi:10.1007/s12103-012-9177-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swami, V., Coles, R., Wilson, E., Salem, N., Wyrozumska, K., & Furnham, A. (2010). Oppressive beliefs at play: Associations among beauty ideals and practices and individual differences in sexism, objectification of others, and media exposure. Psychology Of Women Quarterly, 34(3), 365–379. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2010.01582.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Looy, J., Courtois, C., De Vocht, M., & De Marez, L. (2012). Player identification in online games: Validation of a scale for measuring identification in MMOGs. Media Psychology, 15(2), 197–221. doi:10.1080/15213269.2012.674917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valkenburg, P. M. (2015). The limited informativeness of meta-analyses of media effects. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 10(5), 680–682. doi:10.1177/1745691615592237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wagenmakers, E. J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L., & Kievit, R. A. (2012). An agenda for purely confirmatory research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 632–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williford, A., Boulton, A. J., Forrest-Bank, S. S., Bender, K. A., Dieterich, W. A., & Jenson, J. M. (2016). The effect of bullying and victimization on cognitive empathy development during the transition to middle school. Child & Youth Care Forum, 45(4), 525–541. doi:10.1007/s10566-015-9343-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittek, C. T., Finserås, T. R., Pallesen, S., Mentzoni, R. A., Hanss, D., Griffiths, M. D., & Molde, H. (2016). Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: A study based on a national representative sample of gamers. International Journal Of Mental Health And Addiction, 14(5), 672–686. doi:10.1007/s11469-015-9592-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This reanalysis of a preexisting data set did not have external funding support.

Author Contributions

Both authors contributed to the study conceptualization, data analysis and writing of the final draft manuscript. Both authors read and approved the final version of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher J. Ferguson.

Ethics declarations

Compliance with Ethical Standards

All procedures described within were developed to comport with APA standards for ethical human participant research.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

The original procedures described within received local ethical approval as described in Gabbiadini et al. 2016.

Informed Consent

The original procedures described within were conducted with informed consent provided to participants as described in Gabbiadini et al. 2016.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ferguson, C.J., Donnellan, M.B. Are Associations Between “Sexist” Video Games and Decreased Empathy Toward Women Robust? A Reanalysis of Gabbiadini et al. 2016. J Youth Adolescence 46, 2446–2459 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0700-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0700-x

Keywords

Navigation